Geographic Range
Bonobos (
Pan paniscus
) live in the forests located centrally in the Democratic Republic of the Congo (formerly
Zaire). Bonobo habitat lies in the Congo Basin. This area is located south of an arc
formed by the Congo River (formerly the Zaire River) and its headwater, the Lualaba
River, and north of the Kasai Rver.
Habitat
Within the Congo Basin, bonobos inhabit several vegetation types. The area generally
is classified as tropical rainforest; however, local agriculture and areas reverted
to forest from agriculture (âyoungâ and âaged secondary forestâ) are intermingled.
Species composition, height, and density of trees are different in each, yet all are
utilized by bonobos. In addition to the forested areas, swamp forests opening into
marsh-grassland areas occur, which are also utilized. Foraging occurs in each type
of habitat, while sleeping occurs in forested areas. Some bonobo populations may have
a preference to sleep in relatively small (15 to 30 m tall) trees, particularly those
found in secondary growth forests.
- Habitat Regions
- tropical
- terrestrial
- Terrestrial Biomes
- forest
- rainforest
- Wetlands
- marsh
Physical Description
Contrary to the implication of one of its common names, "pygmy chimpanzee," this species is not particularly diminutive when compared to common chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ). The "pygmy" modifier may instead refer to its location: it lives in an area inhabited by people often referred to as such.
Unlike its closest cousins ( common chimpanzees ), bonobos are not divided into subspecies. Bonobos are apes about two-thirds the size of humans, with dark hair covering their bodies. The hair is generally longer than in common chimpanzees, and is particularly noticeable on the cheeks, which are relatively hairless in P. troglodytes . The portions of body not covered with hair (i.e. mid-face, hands, feet) are darkly colored throughout life. This contrasts with common chimpanzees, which have lighter skin, particularly during the younger years.
Bonobos are primarily knuckle-walkers, although at times they walk bipedally and do
so more frequently than
P. troglodytes
. Bonobos have longer extremities, particularly hind legs, as compared to common chimpanzees.
Although sexual dimorphism exists with males around 30% heavier (37 to 61 kg, 45 kg
average) than females (27 to 38 kg, 33.2 kg average), bonobos are less sexually dimorphic
than many primates, and skeletons are nearly the same size. Average height is 119
cm for males and 111 cm for femals. Average cranial capacity is 350 cubic centimeters.
- Other Physical Features
- endothermic
- homoiothermic
- bilateral symmetry
- Sexual Dimorphism
- male larger
Reproduction
Bonobos are polygynandrous. Females may be approached by and copulate with, any male
in the group except their sons. However, the mating system may be confused by the
use of sexual activity in these animals as part of social bond formation.
- Mating System
- polygynandrous (promiscuous)
Basic life history traits of bonobos are under-researched. Some of the seminal studies of this species have noted that âbonobos have not yet been studied long enough to provide data on age at sexual maturity or birth intervalâ (Nishida and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, 1987), the most frequently researched âWamba and Lomako study populations lack long-term demographic dataâ (Thompson-Handler et al., 1984), and âinformation on the demography of wild bonobos is very limited compared to that for chimpanzeesâ (Furuichi et al., 1998).
Female bonobos undergo estrus, marked by distinctive swelling of the perineal tissue
lasting 10 to 20 days. Matings are concentrated during the time of maximal swelling.
Breeding occurs throughout the year. Postpartum amenorrhea lasts less than one year
in bonobos. A female may resume external signs of estrus (i.e. swelling) within a
year of giving birth. At this point, copulation may resume, although these copulations
do not lead to conception, indicating that the female is probably not fertile. During
this period, she continues to lactate until her offspring is weaned at around 4 years.
The average interbirth interval is 4.6 years (4.8 if one only includes live births).
Therefore, lactation may suppress ovulation, but not the outward signs of estrus.
As no study has lasted longer than a bonobo lifespan, total number of offspring per
female is unknown. However, at Wamba, many adult females had four offspring during
the 20 year study length.
Adult female bonobos have an estrus period that is marked externally by physical changes
in their genitalia. During this time, males of the group approache the female, displaying
their erect penises. Females are generally receptive, and will move toward a male
to allow copulation. There is no clear pattern of mate choice: females are courted
by many males of the group during estrus, with the exception of their sons. Because
of this, paternity is generally unknown to both partners.
- Key Reproductive Features
- iteroparous
- year-round breeding
- gonochoric/gonochoristic/dioecious (sexes separate)
- sexual
- fertilization
- viviparous
Information is limited on parental investment. However, bonobos are highly social mammals and live around 15 years before achieving full adult status. During this time, the mother provides most of the parenting, although the males may contribute indirectly (i.e. in alerting the group of danger, sharing food, and possibly helping to protect young).
Bonobo babies are born relatively helpless. They are dependent on mothersâ milk and cling to their mother for several months. Parental care is provided by the mother, as paternity is generally unclear. Weaning is a gradual process, and is usually commenced by the time the offspring is 4 years of age. Throughout the weaning process, mothers generally have their offspring feed by their side, allowing them to observe the feeding process and food choice, rather than providing them with food directly. Weaning may be enforced by a motherâs refusal to allow a juvenile into her nest, thereby encouraging it to build a nest of its own.
As adults, male bonobos typically remain in their natal social group, so they have
contact with their mothers throughout her remaining years. Female offspring leave
their natal group during late adolescence, so they do not maintain contact with their
mothers in adulthood.
- Parental Investment
- altricial
-
pre-fertilization
-
protecting
- female
-
protecting
-
pre-hatching/birth
-
provisioning
- female
-
protecting
- female
-
provisioning
-
pre-weaning/fledging
-
provisioning
- female
-
protecting
- male
- female
-
provisioning
-
pre-independence
-
protecting
- male
- female
-
protecting
- post-independence association with parents
- extended period of juvenile learning
- inherits maternal/paternal territory
- maternal position in the dominance hierarchy affects status of young
Lifespan/Longevity
Limited information exists on bonobo longevity, and there has been no ongoing study
that lasted longer than the expected bonobo lifespan. The longest semi-continuous
study of bonobos began at Wamba in 1976. At that time, the age of each individual
was estimated, and from extrapolation, a female that died in 1993 was in the 45 to
50 year age range when she died. This would make the lifespan of these animals comparable
to that of
common chimpanzees
.
Behavior
Bonobos are social animals and travel in groups of mixed company: males, females,
and juvenile offspring. Typically, bonobos travel and feed groups of 3 to 6 individuals,
but groups may have as many as 10. At Wamba (a research site where sugar cane fields
that can accommodate many individuals are numerous and provisioning with sugar cane
is also practiced), group size is much larger, and often reaches 30 individuals. Throughout
their range, bonobos temporarily gather in larger groups around plentiful foods, but
fission into smaller groups as they move on. The pattern is similar to that of the
fission-fusion dynamic of
P. troglodytes
, with group size generally limited by availability of certain food items.
Males have a loose dominance structure. Male bonobos stay in their natal group for
life, whereas females leave during adolescence to join another group. A male bonoboâs
rise in dominance is correlated with a motherâs strong presence in the group. Dominance
manifests itself via threat displays, and is frequently associated with gaining access
to food. Most threat displays are unidirectional (the âoffenderâ backs down without
challenging). Afterward, reassurance behaviors minimize any lingering tension. Older
female bonobos gain social status as their male offspring rise in dominance.
Although bonobos are generally knuckle-walkers, they walk bipedally on occasion. Further, they are agile in trees, both climbing and swinging or leaping between branches.
During rest periods, grooming is a common activity. It occurs most frequently between
a male and female, though often between two females. It has been interpreted as not
a greeting, courtship, or tension-relieving behavior, but rather as an âinter-individual
affinityâ or group cohesion activity.
A major focus of research on bonobos centers around their use sexual behaviors in
non-reproductive contexts. These non-copulatory behaviors include female-female contact
(genito-genito-rubbing, or GG rubbing), male-male contact (mounting and rump contact),
and a long period of juvenile and adolescent copulation mimicry (albeit without intromission).
A large amount of the research has been to document the frequency of each behavior
between every pair of group members. These behaviors are observed in females particularly
upon entering a new group after leaving their natal group, and at feeding locales
where a large quantity of food is encountered. These sexual behaviors may be a way
of negotiating and enforcing status differences within both females and males.
Home Range
Bonobo populations have been observed over ranges of between 14 and 29 square kilometers.
However, these reflect observational data, rather than an attempt to map the home
range size of any particular group.
Communication and Perception
Bonobos communicate in a variety of ways. Females have a scream, but males bark, grunt, and pant-hoot. A bark may indicate alarm, whereas other vocalizations may indicate aggression, excitement, satisfaction, etc. The separate types of calls are used in multiple contexts, and cannot be thought of as "words".
In addition to this vocal communication, tactile communication is important. Social rank is communicated by GG rubbing, mounting, or rump contact. (See behavior section.) Other forms of tactile communication are seen between mothers and their offspring, and between rivals.
Visual communication also occurs. Bonobos often "peer" at another individual. This
behavior indicates interest in the activity of the "peered at" individual. Peering
may occur when oneother bonobo has a food item that is wanted, or it may be included
in the courtship behavior of a male.
Food Habits
Fruit comprises the largest portion of the diet of
P. paniscus
, although bonobos incorporate a wide variety of other food items into their diet.
Plant parts consumed include fruit, nuts, stems, shoots, pith, leaves, roots, tubers
and flowers. Mushrooms are also occasionally consumed. Invertebrates form a small
proportion of the diet and include termites, grubs, and worms. On rare occasions,
bonobos have been known to eat meat. They have been directly observed eating flying
squirrels (
Anomalurus
sp.), duiker (
Cephalophus dorsalis
and
Cephalophus nigrifrons
), and bats (
Eidolon
sp.).
- Animal Foods
- mammals
- eggs
- insects
- terrestrial worms
- Plant Foods
- leaves
- roots and tubers
- wood, bark, or stems
- seeds, grains, and nuts
- fruit
- flowers
- Other Foods
- fungus
Predation
The only verified predators of bonobos are humans. Although the hunting of bonobos
is illegal, poaching is still common. It has been speculated that leopards and pythons,
known to prey on common chimpanzees, may also feed on bonobos.
Ecosystem Roles
The quantity of fruit consumed by bonobos suggests that they may play a role in dispersal of the species eaten.
- Ecosystem Impact
- disperses seeds
- non known
- none known
- none known
Economic Importance for Humans: Positive
Bonobos and their sister species, common chimpanzees, are the closest relatives to Homo sapiens . They are an invaluable source of information in studying human origins and diseases.
Bonobos are endearing to humans as 'charasmatic megafauna' and may be useful in encouraging conservation for habitat preservation.
Bonobos continue to be a source of bush meat for human consumption, and although hunting
bonobos has been legally outlawed, poaching continues.
- Positive Impacts
- food
- research and education
Economic Importance for Humans: Negative
Bonobos may eat sugarcane that is being grown for profit. However, direct references to this being a problem for humans have not been encountered in the literature.
Bonobos, similar to common chimpanzees, carry many of the same diseases that can afflict
humans, such as polio.
- Negative Impacts
-
injures humans
- carries human disease
- crop pest
Conservation Status
Bonobos are an endangered species according to both IUCN and US Federal Endangered
Species lists. The IUCN criteria project a 50% or greater reduction in their numbers
within three generations, due to both exploitation and habitat destruction. Bonobos
face âa very high risk of extinction in the wild in the near futureâ according to
the IUCN Red List criteria. Civil war and its aftermath have hampered conservation
efforts. Population estimates vary widely as conflict has limited the ability of researchers
to work in the region. Estimates range from 5,000 to 17,000 individuals left.
Additional Links
Contributors
Nancy Shefferly (editor), Animal Diversity Web.
Anna Williams (author), University of Michigan-Ann Arbor, Phil Myers (editor, instructor), Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor.
- Ethiopian
-
living in sub-Saharan Africa (south of 30 degrees north) and Madagascar.
- native range
-
the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.
- tropical
-
the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.
- terrestrial
-
Living on the ground.
- forest
-
forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.
- rainforest
-
rainforests, both temperate and tropical, are dominated by trees often forming a closed canopy with little light reaching the ground. Epiphytes and climbing plants are also abundant. Precipitation is typically not limiting, but may be somewhat seasonal.
- marsh
-
marshes are wetland areas often dominated by grasses and reeds.
- endothermic
-
animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.
- bilateral symmetry
-
having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.
- polygynandrous
-
the kind of polygamy in which a female pairs with several males, each of which also pairs with several different females.
- iteroparous
-
offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).
- year-round breeding
-
breeding takes place throughout the year
- sexual
-
reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female
- fertilization
-
union of egg and spermatozoan
- viviparous
-
reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.
- altricial
-
young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.
- arboreal
-
Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.
- diurnal
-
- active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
- motile
-
having the capacity to move from one place to another.
- sedentary
-
remains in the same area
- social
-
associates with others of its species; forms social groups.
- dominance hierarchies
-
ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates
- visual
-
uses sight to communicate
- tactile
-
uses touch to communicate
- acoustic
-
uses sound to communicate
- chemical
-
uses smells or other chemicals to communicate
- visual
-
uses sight to communicate
- tactile
-
uses touch to communicate
- acoustic
-
uses sound to communicate
- chemical
-
uses smells or other chemicals to communicate
- food
-
A substance that provides both nutrients and energy to a living thing.
- herbivore
-
An animal that eats mainly plants or parts of plants.
- frugivore
-
an animal that mainly eats fruit
References
Badrian, A., N. Badrian. 1984. Social organization of Pan paniscus in the Lomako Forest, Zaire. Pp. 325-346 in The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Badrian, N., R. Malenky. 1984. Feeding ecology of Pan paniscus in the Lomako Forest, Zaire. Pp. 275-299 in The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Boesch, C. 2002. Behavioural diversity in Pan . Pp. 1-8 in Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos . Cambridge, UK: The Press Cyndicate of the University of Cambridge.
Dahl, J. 1986. Cyclic perineal swelling during the intermenstrual intervals of captive female pygmy chimpanzees ( Pan paniscus ). Journal of Human Evolution , 15: 369-385.
Fruth, B., G. Hohmann. 1993. Ecological and behavioral aspects of nest building in wild bonobos ( Pan paniscus ). Ethology , 94: 113-126.
Furuichi, T., G. Idani, H. Ihobe, S. Kuroda, K. Kitamura, A. Mori, T. Enomoto, N. Okayasu, C. Hashimoto, T. Kano. 1998. Population dynamics of wild bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) at Wamba. International Journal of Primatology , 19/6: 1029-1043.
Furuichi, T. 1987. Sexual swelling, receptivity, and grouping of wild pygmy chimpanzee females at Wamba, Zaire. Primates , 23/3: 309-318.
Furuichi, T. 1989. Social interactions and the life history of female Pan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. International Journal of Primatology , 10/3: 173-197.
Horn, A. 1980. Some observations on the ecology of the bonobo chimpanzee ( Pan paniscus , Schwarz 1929) Near Lake Tumba, Zaire. Folia primatologica , 34: 145-169.
Ihobe, H. 1992. Male-male relationships among wild bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) at Wamba, Republic of Zaire. Primates , 33/2: 163-179.
Ihobe, H. 1992. Observations on the meat-eating behavior of wild bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) at Wamba, Republic of Zaire. Primates , 33/2: 247-250.
Jungers, W., R. Susman. 1984. Body size and skeletal allometry in African apes. Pp. 131-177 in The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Kano, T., M. Mulavwa. 1984. Feeding ecology of the pygmy chimpanzees ( Pan paniscus ) of Wamba. Pp. 233-274 in The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Kano, T. 1992. The Last Ape: Pygmy Chimpanzee Behavior and Ecology . Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Kano, T. 1979. A pilot study on the ecology of pygmy chimpanzees, Pan paniscus . Pp. 123-135 in The Great Apes: Perspectives on Human Evolution, Volume V , Vol. 5. Menlo Park, CA: The Benjamin/Cummings Publishing Company, Inc..
Kano, T. 1983. An ecological study of the pygmy chimpanzees ( Pan paniscus ) of Yalosidi, Republic of Zaire. International Journal of Primatology , 4/1: 1-31.
Kano, T. 1996. Male rank order and copulation rate in a unit-group of bonobos at Wamba, Zaire. Pp. 135-155 in Great Ape Societies . Cambridge, UK: Press Syndicate of the University of Cambridge.
Kano, T. 1980. Social behavior of wild pygmy chimpanzees ( Pan paniscus ) of Wamba: A preliminary report. Journal of Human Evolution , 9: 243-260.
Kano, T. 1982. The social group of pygmy chimpanzees ( Pan paniscus ) of Wamba. Primates , 23/2: 171-188.
Nishida, T., M. Hiraiwa-Hasegawa. 1987. Chimpanzees and bonobos: cooperative relationships among males. Pp. 165-177 in Primate Societies . Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E., B. Wilkerson. 1978. Socio-sexual behavior in Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes : A comparative study. Journal of Human Evolution , 7: 327-344.
Thompson-Handler, N., R. Malenky, N. Badrian. 1984. Sexual behavior of Pan paniscus under natural cnditions in the Lomako Forest, Equaeteur, Zaire. Pp. 347-368 in The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Uehara, S. 1988. Grouping patterns of wild pygmy chimpanzees ( Pan pansicus ) observed at a marsh grassland amidst the tropical rain forest of Yalosidi, Republic of Zaire. Primates , 29/1: 41-52.
Uehara, S. 1990. Utilization patterns of a marsh grassland within the tropical rain forest by the bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) of Yalosidi, Republic of Zaire. Primates , 31/3: 311-322.
Van Krunkelsven, E. 2001. Density estimation of bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) in Salonga National Park, Congo. Biological Conservation , 99: 387-391.
Zihlman, A. 1984. Body build and tissue composition in Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes , with comparisons to other hominoids. Pp. 179-200 in The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum Press.