Geographic Range
Progne subis
, commonly known as purple martins, inhabits the Nearctic region and can be found
across North and South America. They are migratory birds that breed in North and Central
America and overwinter in South America. The northern extent of the breeding range
includes the Canadian provinces of Alberta, Saskatchewan, and southern Manitoba. Purple
martins breed across the eastern half of the United States, and also may be found
along the Pacific coastline including the entire Baja Peninsula.
Purple martins overwinter across most of South America including the countries of
Colombia, Venezuela, Guyana, Suriname, Ecuador, Peru, Brazil, Bolivia, Paraguay and
Argentina. Their winter range has been recorded to be South American lowlands anywhere
east of the Andes Mountains. Concentrated populations have been found to winter in
Bolivia and some provinces of Brazil. There have been some records of purple martin
populations found in the British Isles, but these birds rarely migrate outside of
the Americas.
- Biogeographic Regions
- nearctic
- neotropical
Habitat
Purple martins prefer open spaces that are situated close to any water source, as
they are insectivores and are attracted to the large populations of insects near wetlands,
swamps, and wet meadows. Purple martins also seem to avoid high elevations, for instance
the Appalachian Mountains, but may be found at elevations from less than 100 m to
4,000 m. Due to colonization and human interactions in their natural habitats, purple
martins are now accustomed to human interaction and live in close proximity with humans
today. They tend to find shelter in urban settlements, often living in specially made
birdhouses called "martin houses". Historically, this species inhabited forest edges,
montane forests, and deserts and nested in abandoned woodpecker cavities. Some populations
that breed in the western United States continue to live in these natural settings,
however most utilize man-made martin houses.
During migration, these birds stopover in a variety of habitats. They usually fly
over coastal lines and cross the Gulf of Mexico. They have been recorded in lowlands
and the high mountain ranges of Venezuela and Columbia. They are often seen in cities
and open areas while migrating south. Wintering habitats include rainforests, agricultural
areas, and clearings of South America. They may also reside in urban plazas.
- Habitat Regions
- temperate
- tropical
- terrestrial
- Terrestrial Biomes
- desert or dune
- savanna or grassland
- forest
- mountains
- Aquatic Biomes
- lakes and ponds
- rivers and streams
- temporary pools
- Other Habitat Features
- urban
- suburban
- agricultural
- riparian
Physical Description
Purple martins are the largest North American swallow, measuring 20.3 cm long and
weighing 56 g on average. Their wingspan is about 45.7 cm. Males are a bit larger,
entirely shiny, and deep purple or almost black in coloration. Purple martin males
have less shine on their blackish wings and tails compared to their heads and backs.
Females are overall gray or gray-blue with darker wings and crown feathers, and feature
a white breast smudged with varying degrees of gray. Western females are overall paler
than eastern. Purple martins have a dark, black-brown bill and the average length
is 8.2 mm for males and 8.5 mm for females. Their gape tends to be yellow in young
birds and a dull orangey-brown color in adults. Their legs and feet have a black brown
coloration and their eyes are dark brown.
Juvenile purple martins are overall gray to black with a white belly and gray-streaked
breast. In juveniles, there is a much clearer line between their gray throats and
white bellies compared to females which will have a primarily gray belly. In flight,
a juvenile's tail may have a narrow, slight fork whereas adult tails are distinctly
forked and wider.
Populations residing in the southwest United States exhibit lighter coloration than
purple martins in other regions. This lighter coloration is hypothesized to be an
adaptation to the desert climate and serves to absorb less heat.
- Other Physical Features
- endothermic
- homoiothermic
- bilateral symmetry
- Sexual Dimorphism
- male larger
- sexes colored or patterned differently
- male more colorful
Reproduction
Purple martins are socially monogamous, but form a pair bond that rarely extends to
subsequent seasons. In Texas, 5% of purple martins that occupied more than one nest
practiced polygamy. Males arrive at the breeding grounds first and will select nest
cavities where he will display to potential mates. The male often selects two nest
sites for females to choose from. Males are very territorial and will aggressively
defend nest sites from other males. Unpaired males will perform an aerial display
to any nearby female. This display begins with the male flying out from his nest
in a wide arc, then swooping back into the cavity, popping out his head and singing.
Research has suggested that despite these efforts, females are more interested in
the quality of a nest site than a male's displaying ability. Once a pair has formed,
the male defends the nest cavity as well as his mate. Although the pair is aggressive
towards foreign intruders, they tolerate each other and will continue to tolerate
each other in subsequent years even when they are paired with other mates.
- Mating System
- monogamous
Generally purple martins spend their winters in South America just past the Andes,
and start returning to their northern breeding grounds as early as January 1. They
slowly move northwards progressing generally 3 to 5 degrees latitude (330 to to 550
km) every half a month and they reach their northern limits around May 1. Adult males
often return first, followed shortly by adult females while sub-adults return a couple
weeks after. This general breeding pattern has been found to be true for a generalized
population, except the non-colonial Saguaro desert broods. These purple martins arrive
early May, which is about two and a half months later than any other colony at the
same latitude. Reasons for the difference in arrival patterns have not yet been discovered.
After a pair bond is formed, the martins can start building a nest. Nest building
starts about a month before the pair intends to lay the eggs. Common materials used
to build nests are green leaves, grass, sticks, paper, mud, and feathers. The use
of green leaves as nesting materials is poorly understood, but there are currently
many hypotheses. The female performs most of the construction, while the male gathers
materials and defends the cavity from other martins. Historically, purple martins
used natural cavities to nest in but due to deforestation and the removal of dead
trees, these birds mostly nest within man-made "martin houses" that can house an entire
colony of martins.
The breeding season for purple martins starts in May and will last until June. A purple
martin nest can have anywhere from 3 to 8 white, oval eggs but the average amount
of eggs laid is 5. These eggs are usually about 2.4 by 1.7 cm in size and are then
incubated for 15 to 18 days. The chicks fledge after 26 to 31 days and travel in a
family group. The group returns to sleep at the nest for several days. At 7 to 10
days after fledging the young are able to survive on their own and will disperse.
Young purple martins can reproduce in the first subsequent breeding season.
- Key Reproductive Features
- iteroparous
- seasonal breeding
- gonochoric/gonochoristic/dioecious (sexes separate)
- sexual
- oviparous
After the eggs are laid, the female is the primary incubator. Incubation lasts from
15 to 18 days. When the female leaves the nest, the male will proceed in incubating
the eggs himself but this happens infrequently. As soon as the chicks hatch, brooding
begins and usually lasts until the tenth day. The female alone broods the young. Purple
martin chicks are altricial and are completely dependent upon the parents for survival.
The young are fed within hours of hatching and will continue to be fed for 5 to 7
days after the young fledge. Both the male and female feed the brood. Feeding occurs
by regurgitating food and transferring it into the mouths of the young one by one.
As the brood gets older the feeding sessions become more frequent and reaches its
peak when the young start gaining the most weight, which occurs around days 17 to
21. The feeding becomes so regular that it may occur every 30 seconds. Parents ensure
that the food is the proper size to be swallowed and if the pieces are too large and
swallowing does not occur instantly, the food is removed from their mouths. Parents
keep the nest clean by eating the fecal sacs and encouraging the young to defecate
by poking at their cloacal region. After two weeks of development, the female purple
martin will cease to sleep in the same compartment as the nestlings, because less
frequent night brooding is necessary.
- Parental Investment
- altricial
- male parental care
- female parental care
-
pre-fertilization
- provisioning
-
protecting
- male
-
pre-hatching/birth
-
provisioning
- male
- female
-
protecting
- male
- female
-
provisioning
-
pre-weaning/fledging
-
provisioning
- male
- female
-
protecting
- male
- female
-
provisioning
-
pre-independence
-
provisioning
- male
- female
-
protecting
- male
- female
-
provisioning
Lifespan/Longevity
The longevity of purple martins range from 0 to 13 years and nine months. Purple martin
mortality is often the result of severe weather. Three to four days of severe weather
can lead to insect numbers drastically declining. If there is a lack of food, purple
martins cannot survive and this often results in population decline. Another hindrance
to long life expectancy is often body parasites. Purple martins host a protozoan blood
parasite
Haemoproteus prognei
. This parasite can have disastrous effects on the surviving rate of first year birds
during the winter and migration period.
Behavior
Purple martins are migratory birds that travel from central North America to northern
South America to breed and overwinter, respectively. They are active during the day
and are most active at dawn and dusk. They are colonial birds that live together in
large colonies. They either choose to fly around in pairs or stay in their colonial
groups. At the end of the summer the whole colony commutes back to South America.
These communal flights home can include tens of thousands of birds.
Purple martins have shown distinct group behavior. Martin pairs are monogamous and
very territorial, but when they join a colony they can show strong group-orientation
and protectiveness of the colony's territory. The colony socializes together by exploring
the environment around them. This includes visiting other colonies, scoping out potential
food sources, and visiting nearby streams and swamps. The socializing groups consist
of 3 or more purple martins. Purple martins forage together by hunting in large flocks.
When the flocks do find food they call the rest of the colony to come feed by making
distinct sounds.
Purple martins also display synchrony when they live in large colonies. Colonial
purple martins work together in an effort to increase the reproductive success of
the colony. Larger colonies have more success at achieving synchrony, because they
are usually made up of mature birds, where as smaller colonies are made up of younger
adults with less experience. Synchrony is related to age and is reflected better in
birds of similar age. A colony of purple martins will achieve synchrony better if
the birds are similar in age. Colonial living does carry the risks of increased disease
transmission and resource competition.
Purple martins are known to act aggressively when they are threatened. For instance,
they will defend themselves by pecking and flapping their wings trying to encourage
the unwanted visitor to leave. When purple martins are threatened they take on a āHorizontal
Threat Postureā, orientating their bodies so that it runs parallel with the surface
and pressing their feathers down close to their bodies. Often when threatened, purple
martins alert other martins by singing. Another defense mechanism purple martins resort
to is gaping. Gaping is when a purple martin opens its mouth wide and directs it to
the potential threat. Purple martins resort to lunging as an intimidation effect and
will also try to intimidate a threat by bill snapping.
- Key Behaviors
- arboreal
- flies
- glides
- diurnal
- crepuscular
- motile
- migratory
- territorial
- social
- colonial
Home Range
Purple martins often return to the same nesting site where they had lived the previous
year. There is no information on how far these birds will travel while foraging. Purple
martins have the ability to fly far distances to get to their nests. This suggests
that these birds must have a large home range. Purple martins are known to travel
from 2.8 to 664 km to get back to their nests.
Communication and Perception
Purple martins communicate vocally and visually. Purple martins have eleven identified
vocalizations that they use for different occasions like mating, warning, and teaching
the young during fledging. Male purple martins use singing and visual displays to
attract potential female mates. Juvenile purple martins resort to āchoo-chooā calls
to attract their parentsā attention if protection is needed. Parents use juvenile
'choo-choo' calls to assemble their broods and return them safely to the nest. Female
purple martins use the āchooā call to lead their young to and from groupings areas
during the fledging period. Purple martins only resort to āzwarckā calls when they
need to send a high intensity alarm, and it is often accompanied by the birds diving
straight down towards the invader. Male purple martins use āhee-heeā vocalizations
to fight off intruders. āZweetā calls are used to show intraspecific excitement, as
well as send an alarm to warn other purple martins of a potential threat and to encourage
them to fly away. Purple martins use ācherā calls to communicate daily and will use
āchortleā calls in high excitement situations. Males attract females by singing ācroak
songsā as well as to warn off unmated males from entering their territory. During
courtship males make a clicking sound by snapping their lower and upper mandibles
together. The last vocalization male purple martins use are āsubsongsā and are heard
during feeding and pre-migratory periods. āSubsongsā are used to communicate with
other purple martins while socializing together. Purple martin males that are part
of a stable colony often perform a 'dawnsong' which include a variety of sounds early
in the morning.
Food Habits
Purple martins are primarily insectivores. They capture food in flight and rarely
glean insects from foliage or the ground. Purple martins prefer eating fruit flies
(
Ceratitis
), mosquitoes (
Culicidae
), wasps (
Polistes
), beetles (
Coleoptera
), ants (
Formicidae
), grasshoppers (
Orthoptera
), cicadas (
Cicadidae
) as well as dragonflies (
Anisoptera
). Purple martins may consume 400 flies or 2000 mosquitoes in a day.
Purple martins rarely eat spiders
Araneida
and prefer any other insect instead. Generally purple martin diets consists of 23%
wasps and bees (
Hymenoptera
), 16% flies, 15% assorted bugs like stink bugs (
Pentatomidae
) and black bugs (
Thyreocoridae
), and 12% were beetles (
Onthophagus
). Purple martins also eat butterflies and moths (
Lepidoptera
) but dragonflies seem to be preferred. Young purple martins prefer eating dragonflies
over other insects while adults show no specific preference to dragonflies.
Purple martins are greatly dependent on the weather, since it has a profound effect
on insect populations. At low temperatures insect food sources tend to decrease, where
as at high temperature purple martins have an abundance of food. High velocity winds
also decrease food availability. Purple martins tend to eat beetles throughout all
the seasons, but flies tend to disappear from their diet in late August. Insect populations
tend to be at its highest during August which coincides with greater nutritional need
in preparation for fall migration. It is during August that purple martins must hoard
up food and nutrients for the long flight back south. These birds consume water in
flight by skimming their lower beaks across any water source.
- Primary Diet
- carnivore
- Animal Foods
- insects
- terrestrial non-insect arthropods
Predation
The most common predators for purple martins are
owls
and
snakes
which prey on both adults and juveniles. Owls attack while the birds are inside the
nest. Owls grab the nest and shake it, which disorientates the purple martin. Purple
martins then try to slip out of the entrance hole, but owls grabs them with their
claw. Humans can help prevent these owl attacks by attaching curved rods over nest
entrances so that owls cannot perch atop the martin houses. Owls have also been known
to reach their claws into martin houses to grab purple martins.
Predators like
snakes
or
raccoons
are able to climb the bird house poles and make their way to the entrance cavities.
They pull out any adult birds and then proceed to eat the eggs. The snakes that tend
to prey on purple martins are usually non-poisonous and often climb up the poles and
eat both the eggs and young.
Rat snakes
are the most common snake predators.
Hawks
and
blue herons
are the only predators that prey on purple martins in the air.
Domestic cats
prey on purple martins when they are on the ground in search of nesting material.
Squirrels
also prey on purple martins by climbing up the nest and entering the cavity. Squirrels
kill the young, break up all the eggs and can even occupy the nest to raise their
own young.
One anti-predation behavior shown by purple martins is vigilant nest cleaning. Purple
martin parents will eat fecal sacs and encourage juvenile birds to defecate by poking
at their cloacal region. The feces will be either consumed or removed from the nest
by dropping them outside. The elimination of feces and fecal sacs allows for protection,
since the scent trails would be removed.
Purple martins respond to predator attacks by sending the 'zweet' call. 'Zweet' calls
are used to warn other purple martins of the threat or to encourage them to fly away.
Purple martins often dive bomb their attacker. Purple martin colonies have no coordinated
response to predators. They do not all attack the predator but do assemble as a crowd
to confuse predators and make it difficult to focus on one bird. The only birds that
do attack the predator are the owners of a threatened nest. Purple martins will generally
stay out of the predatorās way unless the predator comes within a few meters of their
nest and young. Then purple martin adults have been known to attack the predator.
Purple martins benefit from living in large colonies, because it adds to their protection
and stability. Large colonies are able to detect predators faster, thereby decreasing
predation.
Ecosystem Roles
Purple martins play roles as predator, prey, competitor, and host. Purple martins
are insectivorous and are known to be effective pest controllers. Purple martins are
preyed upon by many species and also serve as a host to several species of parasites.
Purple martin populations have been greatly affected by mite parasites, specifically
Dermanyssus prognephilus
that live inside their nests. These blood-feeding parasites are able to decrease
clutch size and an outbreak may lead to colony abandonment. Other parasites include
ticks
,
beetles
,
louseflies
,
fleas
, and
bowflies
.
Purple martins have to compete for nesting sites with
house sparrows
and
European starlings
. Starlings often corner purple martins in their own nest cavities where fighting
results and often ends in death. This competition is particularly unfortunate, as
both
house sparrows
and
European starlings
are invasive species in the United States and often out-compete native purple martins
for nesting habitat.
- nest mites ( Dermanyssus prognephilus )
- ticks ( Ornithodoros )
- louseflies ( Hippoboscidae )
- fleas ( Ceratophyllus )
- bowflies ( Protocalliphora hirundo )
- bowflies ( Protocalliphora sialia )
- beetles ( Attagenus piceus )
Economic Importance for Humans: Positive
Humans benefit largely from the insectivorous food habits of purple martins. Purple
martins consume large quantities of pest species including flies, stink bugs, clover
weevil beetles, and mosquitoes.
- Positive Impacts
- controls pest population
Economic Importance for Humans: Negative
There are no known adverse effects of purple martins on humans.
Conservation Status
The IUCN Red List has listed purple martins as a species of least concern. The United
States Federal list has listed purples martins as being a species of concern. Currently
several groups are working on conserving the natural habitat of purple martins. Under
the Michigan Special Animal list, purple martins are not in danger. Overall, purple
martins have stable population numbers and inhabit a wide geographical range. In Canada,
purple martin populations have seen some decline, and are currently considered at
risk in British Columbia. Local populations have suffered greatly from weather related
mortalities in the northern edges of the breeding range. Purple martins are also declining
due to the competition for nesting sites. Purple martins compete with invasive
house sparrows
and
European starlings
for nesting sites. Currently in British Columbia they are setting up special nesting
boxes in the hope of sustaining the remaining population.
Additional Links
Contributors
Nadine Snyman (author), University of Alberta, Augustana Campus, Doris Audet (editor), University of Alberta, Augustana Campus, Rachelle Sterling (editor), Special Projects, Catherine Kent (editor), Special Projects.
- Nearctic
-
living in the Nearctic biogeographic province, the northern part of the New World. This includes Greenland, the Canadian Arctic islands, and all of the North American as far south as the highlands of central Mexico.
- native range
-
the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.
- Neotropical
-
living in the southern part of the New World. In other words, Central and South America.
- native range
-
the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.
- temperate
-
that region of the Earth between 23.5 degrees North and 60 degrees North (between the Tropic of Cancer and the Arctic Circle) and between 23.5 degrees South and 60 degrees South (between the Tropic of Capricorn and the Antarctic Circle).
- tropical
-
the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.
- terrestrial
-
Living on the ground.
- desert or dunes
-
in deserts low (less than 30 cm per year) and unpredictable rainfall results in landscapes dominated by plants and animals adapted to aridity. Vegetation is typically sparse, though spectacular blooms may occur following rain. Deserts can be cold or warm and daily temperates typically fluctuate. In dune areas vegetation is also sparse and conditions are dry. This is because sand does not hold water well so little is available to plants. In dunes near seas and oceans this is compounded by the influence of salt in the air and soil. Salt limits the ability of plants to take up water through their roots.
- tropical savanna and grassland
-
A terrestrial biome. Savannas are grasslands with scattered individual trees that do not form a closed canopy. Extensive savannas are found in parts of subtropical and tropical Africa and South America, and in Australia.
- savanna
-
A grassland with scattered trees or scattered clumps of trees, a type of community intermediate between grassland and forest. See also Tropical savanna and grassland biome.
- temperate grassland
-
A terrestrial biome found in temperate latitudes (>23.5° N or S latitude). Vegetation is made up mostly of grasses, the height and species diversity of which depend largely on the amount of moisture available. Fire and grazing are important in the long-term maintenance of grasslands.
- forest
-
forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.
- mountains
-
This terrestrial biome includes summits of high mountains, either without vegetation or covered by low, tundra-like vegetation.
- marsh
-
marshes are wetland areas often dominated by grasses and reeds.
- swamp
-
a wetland area that may be permanently or intermittently covered in water, often dominated by woody vegetation.
- bog
-
a wetland area rich in accumulated plant material and with acidic soils surrounding a body of open water. Bogs have a flora dominated by sedges, heaths, and sphagnum.
- urban
-
living in cities and large towns, landscapes dominated by human structures and activity.
- suburban
-
living in residential areas on the outskirts of large cities or towns.
- agricultural
-
living in landscapes dominated by human agriculture.
- riparian
-
Referring to something living or located adjacent to a waterbody (usually, but not always, a river or stream).
- endothermic
-
animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.
- bilateral symmetry
-
having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.
- monogamous
-
Having one mate at a time.
- iteroparous
-
offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).
- seasonal breeding
-
breeding is confined to a particular season
- sexual
-
reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female
- oviparous
-
reproduction in which eggs are released by the female; development of offspring occurs outside the mother's body.
- altricial
-
young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.
- male parental care
-
parental care is carried out by males
- female parental care
-
parental care is carried out by females
- arboreal
-
Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.
- diurnal
-
- active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
- crepuscular
-
active at dawn and dusk
- motile
-
having the capacity to move from one place to another.
- migratory
-
makes seasonal movements between breeding and wintering grounds
- territorial
-
defends an area within the home range, occupied by a single animals or group of animals of the same species and held through overt defense, display, or advertisement
- social
-
associates with others of its species; forms social groups.
- colonial
-
used loosely to describe any group of organisms living together or in close proximity to each other - for example nesting shorebirds that live in large colonies. More specifically refers to a group of organisms in which members act as specialized subunits (a continuous, modular society) - as in clonal organisms.
- visual
-
uses sight to communicate
- acoustic
-
uses sound to communicate
- duets
-
to jointly display, usually with sounds in a highly coordinated fashion, at the same time as one other individual of the same species, often a mate
- choruses
-
to jointly display, usually with sounds, at the same time as two or more other individuals of the same or different species
- carnivore
-
an animal that mainly eats meat
- insectivore
-
An animal that eats mainly insects or spiders.
- visual
-
uses sight to communicate
- tactile
-
uses touch to communicate
- acoustic
-
uses sound to communicate
- chemical
-
uses smells or other chemicals to communicate
References
Alsop III, F. 2002. Purple Martin. Pp. "464" in Birds of Canada , Vol. I, 1st Edition. Toronto Ont.: Southern lights Custom Publishing.
Brauning, D. 1992. Atlas of Breeding Birds in Pennsylvania . Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Brown, C. 1978. Post-Fledging Behavior of Purple Martins. The Wilson Bulletin , 90/3: "376-385". Accessed November 01, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/4161087 .
Brown, C. 1980. Sleeping Behavior of Purple Martins. The Condor , 82/2: "170-175". Accessed November 01, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/1367472 .
Brown, C. 1979. Territoriality in the Purple Martin. The Wilson Bulletin , 91/4: "583-593". Accessed November 01, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/4161272 .
Brown, C. 1997. Birds Of North America . Philadelphia, PA: The American Ornithologists/ Union and The Academy of Natural Sciences.
Brown, C. 1984. Vocalizations of the Purple Martin. The Condor , 86/4: 433-442. Accessed October 03, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/1366823 .
Campbell Finlay, J. 1976. Some Effects of Weather on Purple Martin Activity. The Auk , 93/2: "231-244". Accessed October 31, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/4085041 .
Davis, J., C. Brown. 1999. Costs Of Coloniality And The Effect Of Colony Size On Reproductive Success In Purple Martins. The Condor , 101/4: "737-745". Accessed September 16, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/1370060 .
Fraser, D., C. Siddle, D. Copley, E. Walters. 1997. The Return of the Purple Martin in British Columbia. Wildlife Branch, British Columbia Ministry of Environment, Lands and Parks , 1: "279-282". Accessed November 07, 2010 at http://www.env.gov.bc.ca/wld/documents/ce07fraser2.pdf .
Johnston, R. 1966. The Adaptive Basis of Geographic Variation in Color of The Purple Martin. The Condor , 68/3: "219-228". Accessed November 07, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/1365555 .
Johnston, R., J. Hardy. 1962. Behavior of the Purple Martin. The Wilson Bulletin , Vol 64, No. 3: "243-262". Accessed September 25, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/4159068 .
Kostka, K. 2000. Owl Guards for Gourds: A Way to Protect Martin Gourds from Aerial Predators. Purple Martin Update , 9/4: "1".
Layton, R. 1972. The Purple Martin . Jackson, Missisippi: Nature Books Publishers.
Morton, E., K. Derrickson. 1990. The Biological Significance Of Age-specific Return Schedules In Breeding Purple Martins. The Condor , 92: "1040-1050". Accessed September 25, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/pss/1368740 .
Morton, E. 1985. Vocal Imitation in a Captive Purple Martin. The Wilson Bulletin , 97/3: "392-395". Accessed November 01, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/4162123 .
Moss, W., J. Camin. 1970. Nest Parasitism, Productivity, and Clutch Size in Purple Martins. Science , 168/3934: "1000-1003".
Stutchbury, B., J. Hill III, P. Krame, S. Rush, S. Tarof. 2009. Sex And Age-Specific Annual Survival In A Neotropical Migratory Songbird, The Purple Martin (Progne Subis). The Auk , 126/2: "278-287".
Turner, A. 1985. Swallows. Pp. "332-335" in The Encyclopedia of Birds , Vol. I, I Edition. New York: Facts on File.
Wade, J. 1966. What You Should Know About The Purple Martin . Griggsville, Illinois: J. L. Wade.
Wagner, R., M. Schug, E. Morton. 1996. Condition-dependent control of paternity by female purple martins: implications for coloniality. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology , 38/6: "379-389".
Williams, J. 1987. Field Metabolism and Food Consumption of Savannah Sparrows during the Breeding Season. The Auk , 104/2: "277-289". Accessed November 01, 2010 at http://www.jstor.org/stable/4087034 .
U.S Department of Agriculture. Purple Martin Birdhouse. 204,523. New York, NY: Armond Industries, Inc.. 1971.