Psittacus erithacusgrey parrot

Ge­o­graphic Range

African grey par­rots (Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus) span the for­est belt of cen­tral and West Africa in­clud­ing the oceanic is­land of Príncipe (Gulf of Guinea). In West­ern Africa, they are found in coastal coun­tries such as Sierra Leone, Ghana, and the Ivory Coast. The two known sub­species of African Grey Par­rots have vary­ing ranges. Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus erithi­cus in­hab­its a range ex­tend­ing from Kenya to the east­ern bor­der of the Ivory Coast and in­clud­ing the in­su­lar pop­u­la­tions. Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus tim­neh has a range from the east­ern bor­der of Ivory Coast to Guinea-Bis­sau. (Melo and O'Ryan, 2007)

Habi­tat

The habi­tat of African grey par­rots is usu­ally moist low­land forests, al­though they are found up to 2,200 m al­ti­tude in the east­ern parts of the range. They are com­monly ob­served at for­est edges, clear­ings, gallery forests, man­groves, wooded sa­van­nahs, cul­ti­vated areas, and gar­dens. African grey par­rots often visit open land ad­ja­cent to wood­lands, they roost in trees over water and may pre­fer roost­ing on is­lands in rivers. These par­rots make their nests in tree holes, some­times choos­ing lo­ca­tions aban­doned by birds like wood­peck­ers. In West Africa, the species makes sea­sonal move­ments out of the dri­est parts of the range in the dry sea­son. ("Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus, Lin­naeeus, 1758", 2008; Athan and Deter, 2000; Melo and O'Ryan, 2007)

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

The plumage of Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus is var­i­ous shades of grey with very dis­tinc­tive red tail feath­ers. African grey par­rots typ­i­cally mea­sure 33 cm from head to tail and weigh up to 407g. They have an av­er­age wingspan of 46-52 cm. (Athan and Deter, 2000; Melo and O'Ryan, 2007)

Psit­ta­cus e. eritha­cus> is re­ferred to as the nom­i­nate race and is light grey. In­di­vid­u­als of this sub­species have dis­tinct red tails and solid black beaks. These birds have bare white face patches and some­times bright, usu­ally pale, sil­very yel­low eyes. Many of the grey con­tour feath­ers are edged with white. This gives them a smooth, lacy ap­pear­ance. They may be some­what sex­u­ally di­mor­phic. (Athan and Deter, 2000; Melo and O'Ryan, 2007)

Psit­ta­cus e. tim­neh in­di­vid­u­als are smaller and darker with a ma­roon, brown­ish wash over the red tail. They have black-tipped, dark pink­ish max­illa and solid black mandibles. Their iris has more of a sil­ver ap­pear­ance rather than yel­low (Athan and Deter, 2000; Melo and O'Ryan, 2007)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • sexes colored or patterned differently
  • Average mass
    407 g
    14.34 oz
  • Average length
    33 cm
    12.99 in
  • Range wingspan
    18 to 20 cm
    7.09 to 7.87 in

Re­pro­duc­tion

African grey par­rots are very so­cial birds. Breed­ing oc­curs in loose colonies with each pair oc­cu­py­ing its own tree. In­di­vid­u­als se­lect mates care­fully and have a life­long monog­a­mous bond that be­gins at sex­u­ally ma­tu­rity, at three to five years of age. Few de­tails are known about courtship in the wild, but dis­play flights around nest holes have been ob­served and recorded. Males feed mates (courtship feed­ing) and both sing soft mo­not­o­nous notes. At this time the fe­male will sleep in the nest cav­ity while the male guards it. In cap­tiv­ity, males feed fe­males after cop­u­la­tion events and both sexes par­tic­i­pate in a mat­ing dance in which they droop their wings. ("African Grey Par­rots", 2006; "Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus (African Grey Par­rot, Congo African Grey Par­rot, Grey Par­rot)", 2008; Athan, 1999; Pep­per­berg, 2001)

The breed­ing sea­son varies by lo­cal­ity, but ap­pears to co­in­cide with the dry sea­son. African grey par­rots breed once to twice a year. Fe­males lay three to five roundish eggs, one each at in­ter­vals of two to five days. Fe­males in­cu­bate the eggs while being fed en­tirely by the male. In­cu­ba­tion takes ap­prox­i­mately thirty days and the young emerge from the nest at twelve weeks old. ("African Grey Par­rots", 2006; "Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus (African Grey Par­rot, Congo African Grey Par­rot, Grey Par­rot)", 2008; Athan, 1999; Pep­per­berg, 2001)

  • Breeding interval
    African Grey Parrots breed once to twice a year.
  • Breeding season
    Reproduction appears to coincide with the dry season.
  • Range eggs per season
    3 to 5
  • Average time to hatching
    30 days
  • Average fledging age
    12 weeks
  • Range time to independence
    2 to 3 years
  • Average time to independence
    3 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    3 to 5 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    3 to 5 years

After the young emerge from the nest, both par­ents feed, raise, and pro­tect them. Both par­ents care for their clutch of young until they reach in­de­pen­dence. ("African Grey Par­rots", 2006; "Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus (African Grey Par­rot, Congo African Grey Par­rot, Grey Par­rot)", 2008; Athan, 1999; Pep­per­berg, 2001)

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • pre-fertilization
    • provisioning
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • extended period of juvenile learning

Lifes­pan/Longevity

In cap­tive and wild par­rots the av­er­age lifes­pan is be­tween 40 and 50 years. In cap­tiv­ity, African grey par­rots have a mean lifes­pan of 45 years, but they can live up to 60 years. In the wild, the av­er­age lifes­pan is 22.7 years (n=120). ("African Grey Par­rots", 2006; "Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus, Lin­naeeus, 1758", 2008; Ryan, 2002)

  • Range lifespan
    Status: captivity
    40 to 60 years
  • Average lifespan
    Status: captivity
    45 years
  • Average lifespan
    Status: wild
    22.7 years
  • Typical lifespan
    Status: captivity
    40 to 50 years

Be­hav­ior

Wild African grey par­rots are very shy and rarely allow hu­mans to ap­proach them. They are highly so­cial and nest in large groups, al­though fam­ily groups oc­cupy their own nest­ing tree. They are often ob­served roost­ing in large, noisy flocks call­ing loudly dur­ing morn­ings and evenings and in flight. These flocks are com­posed of only African grey par­rots, un­like other par­rots that are often found in mixed flocks. Dur­ing the day, they break into smaller flocks and fly long dis­tances to for­age. They often roost in trees over water and are said to pre­fer roost­ing on is­lands in rivers. Young birds stay with their fam­ily groups for a long pe­riod of time, up to sev­eral years. They so­cial­ize with oth­ers of their age in nurs­ery trees, but re­main in their fam­ily group within the larger flock. Young African grey par­rots are cared for by older birds until they are ed­u­cated enough and old enough to be­come in­de­pen­dent flock mem­bers. Young ex­hibit ap­pease­ment be­hav­iors to­wards older mem­bers. As they ma­ture, birds be­come more ag­gres­sive with con­specifics. African grey par­rots in the wild must learn a com­plex set of skills. They need to learn how to sep­a­rate de­sir­able food plants from toxic plants, how to de­fend ter­ri­tory, how to rec­og­nize and avoid preda­tors, how to find safe water, and how to re­join their fam­i­lies when sep­a­rated. Also, they must learn how to de­velop role-ap­pro­pri­ate be­hav­iors such as com­pet­ing and de­fend­ing nest sites and rais­ing off­spring. Com­pe­ti­tion for nest holes dur­ing mat­ing sea­son makes the species ex­tremely ag­gres­sive. Be­cause African grey par­rots are par­tial ground feed­ers, there is a se­ries of be­hav­ioral events that occur be­fore land­ing and safe con­sump­tion takes place. Groups of par­rots gather at a bar­ren tree until it is com­pletely filled with hun­dreds of birds that par­take in preen­ing, climb­ing, vo­cal­iz­ing, and so­cial­iz­ing. Even­tu­ally the birds make their way down to the ground in waves with the en­tire group never being on the ground at the same time. Once on the ground, they are ex­tremely vig­i­lant, re­act­ing to any move­ment and/or sound. ("African Grey Par­rots", 2006; Athan and Deter, 2000; Athan, 1999; Galef, 2004; Luescher, 2006; Wright, 2002)

Stud­ies have found that African grey par­rots have com­plex cog­ni­tion and are con­sid­ered one of the most in­tel­li­gent species of an­i­mal. They test and form ideas about the world. In a study pre­formed by Irene Pep­per­berg (2007), African grey par­rots were tested on in­sight­ful be­hav­ior and im­i­ta­tive com­pe­tence. The re­sults showed that the two par­rots with lim­ited vo­cab­u­lary im­me­di­ately acted out the cor­rect phys­i­cal, in­sight­ful task. The par­rots that had re­ceived train­ing in ref­er­en­tial Eng­lish speech at­tempted in­stead to ma­nip­u­late their trainer. They en­gaged in de­lib­er­ate com­mu­ni­ca­tion as a prob­lem-solv­ing strat­egy, which is an ad­vanced stage of de­vel­op­ment, even for human in­fants. The most fa­mous African Grey Par­rot, Alex, ex­hib­ited cog­ni­tive ca­pac­i­ties com­pa­ra­ble to those of ma­rine mam­mals, apes, and 4 to 6 year old chil­dren. Some of Alex’s ac­com­plish­ments in­cluded the abil­ity to label 50 dif­fer­ent ob­jects, 7 col­ors, 5 shapes and quan­ti­ties up to and in­clud­ing six. He would com­bine these la­bels to iden­tify, re­quest, refuse, cat­e­go­rize, and quan­tify about 100 dif­fer­ent ob­jects. Alex also had func­tional use of phases and had a con­cept of cat­e­gory. African Grey Par­rots have be­hav­ior pat­terns that match some non-hu­man pri­mates and that par­al­lel com­bi­na­tory de­vel­op­ment. In an ex­per­i­ment per­formed by Galef (2004), African grey par­rots were ob­served to copy a va­ri­ety of ac­tions in­volv­ing six dif­fer­ent body parts and to as­so­ci­ate each ac­tion with its ap­pro­pri­ate label. This is a form of so­cial-learn­ing and im­i­ta­tion. (Galef, 2004; Pep­per­berg, 2001; Pep­per­berg, 2006; Pep­per­berg, 2007)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

Wild African grey par­rot flocks fol­low a daily pat­tern of vo­cal­iza­tions. Usu­ally the flock is quiet from sun­set until the next dawn. At day break, the flock be­gins to vo­cal­ize be­fore set­ting out to for­age at dif­fer­ent lo­ca­tions through­out the day. At dusk, upon re­turn to the roost­ing site, there is a pe­riod of vo­cal­iza­tion. There are a va­ri­ety of dif­fer­ent types of calls and vo­cal­iza­tions, in­clud­ing alarm calls, con­tact calls, food beg­ging calls, and ag­o­nis­tic calls. Con­tact calls are of par­tic­u­lar im­por­tance be­cause they serve to iden­tify where other mem­bers of the flock are and help pro­mote flock co­he­sion. Alarm calls in­di­cate vary­ing lev­els of dis­tress, these calls are par­tic­u­larly loud and of a fre­quency that car­ries well in order to warn fel­low flock mem­bers. Young learn these vo­cal­iza­tions from par­ents and flock mates, so pet par­rots will not learn ap­pro­pri­ate vo­cal­iza­tions, but will show sim­i­lar pat­terns and use of calls. Bot­toni et al. (2003) found that African grey par­rots demon­strated com­plex cog­ni­tive com­pe­tence in un­der­stand­ing both the sim­i­lar­i­ties and dis­sim­i­lar­i­ties among mu­si­cal note fre­quen­cies and were able to mas­ter the mu­si­cal code. It was de­ter­mined that African grey par­rots must iso­late a sound from back­ground noise, im­i­tate it, cat­e­go­rize the acoustic stim­u­lus, en­code it into long term mem­ory, and mon­i­tor the out­put sound to match it with the in­ter­nal tem­plate. The fa­mous African grey par­rot, Alex, achieved a rudi­men­tary form of com­mu­ni­ca­tion, in­clud­ing con­tex­tual and con­cep­tual use of human speech. That re­search showed that African grey par­rots are ca­pa­ble of far more than sim­ply mim­ic­ing human speech. (Bot­toni, et al., 2003; Luescher, 2006; Pep­per­berg, 2000)

Food Habits

African grey par­rots are her­bi­vores. In the wild, they feed pri­mar­ily on nuts and fruits, sup­ple­mented by leafy mat­ter, fruits, in­sects, bark, and flow­ers. African grey par­rots eat mostly com­mon fruits, such as oil-palm (Elaeis guinen­sis). (Athan and Deter, 2000; Faye, 2006)

  • Animal Foods
  • insects
  • Plant Foods
  • leaves
  • seeds, grains, and nuts
  • fruit
  • flowers

Pre­da­tion

Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus are ha­rassed and preyed on by palm-nut vul­tures (Gy­po­hierax an­golen­sis). Sev­eral species of hawks also prey on fledg­lings and adults. Mon­keys prey on eggs and young in nests. When feed­ing on the ground, African grey par­rots are vul­ner­a­ble to ter­res­trial preda­tors. ("African Grey Par­rots", 2006; Athan and Deter, 2000)

Ecosys­tem Roles

African grey par­rots may dis­perse the seeds of fruits they eat. They act as de­fin­i­tive hosts to both tape­worms and blood par­a­sites. ("African Grey Par­rots", 2006; Athan and Deter, 2000)

Com­men­sal/Par­a­sitic Species
  • tape­worms (Ces­toda)
  • blood par­a­sites

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

African grey par­rots are the sec­ond most heav­ily har­vested par­rot in the world. The trade be­tween 1980 and 1995 doc­u­mented an ex­cess of 500,000 birds caught in the wild. From 1994 to 2003, just fewer than 360,000 wild caught par­rots were re­port­edly ex­ported from their na­tive range. They are one of the most pop­u­lar avian pets in Eu­rope, the United States, and the Mid­dle East. In Principe, trap­pers heav­ily har­vest African grey par­rots for the in­ter­na­tional pet trade. ("Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus, Lin­naeeus, 1758", 2008; Fahlman, 2002; Juste, 1995; Melo and O'Ryan, 2007)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

There are no known ad­verse af­fects of Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus on hu­mans.

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus is con­sid­ered to be a near threat­ened species be­cause of a re­cent analy­sis sug­gest­ing that up to 21% of the global pop­u­la­tion may be har­vested an­nu­ally. The quota for African grey par­rots in the De­mo­c­ra­tic Re­pub­lic of the Congo is 5000, 4000 in Congo, and 250 in Gabon. Un­for­tu­nately, there is no law pro­hibit­ing cap­ture and trade of par­rots. These birds are im­pacted by habi­tat de­struc­tion, in­dis­crim­i­nate use of pes­ti­cides, and hunt­ing by local in­hab­i­tants. Trap­ping for the wild bird trade is a major cause of de­cline in wild African grey par­rots pop­u­la­tions. ("CITES species data­base", 2008; "The IUCN Red List of Threat­ened Species", 2008)

Other Com­ments

In cap­tiv­ity, African grey par­rots often suf­fer med­ical prob­lems such as obe­sity, hy­povi­t­a­minosis A, hypocal­cemia, amino acid im­bal­ance, or mi­cromin­eral de­fi­cien­cies. Prob­lems are often as­so­ci­ated with poor diets com­posed of seeds and nuts with sup­ple­mented fruits. These food sources con­tain high lev­els of car­bo­hy­drates. African grey par­rots can also have cal­cium de­fi­cien­cies which can lead to seizures. They can suf­fer from res­pi­ra­tory ail­ments caused by fun­gal dis­eases, bac­te­r­ial in­fec­tions/pneu­mo­nia, and nu­tri­tional de­fi­cien­cies. Other health is­sues ob­served are; hy­per­k­er­a­totic swellings, ma­lig­nant tu­mors, tape­worm and blood par­a­sites. They are also sus­cep­ti­ble to Psittacine Beak and feather Dis­ease (PBFD). ("African Grey Par­rots", 2006; "Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus, Lin­naeeus, 1758", 2008; Ryan, 2002)

In cap­tiv­ity, African grey par­rots are prone to cer­tain be­hav­ioral prob­lems. For ex­am­ple, feather pick­ing in re­sponse to so­cial stress. Ter­ri­tory is­sues can also arise and are more com­mon in males than fe­males. (Athan and Deter, 2000; Faye, 2006)

Cap­tive African grey par­rots thrive on a diet of seeds, grain, pel­lets, and fruits and veg­eta­bles. It is sug­gested that pet own­ers sup­ple­ment the diet with cal­cium in the form of dark leafy greens, cal­cium-rich veg­eta­bles, fruits, oat­meal, al­monds, wal­nuts, hazel­nuts, and sesame seeds. Flax seed and sun­flower seed are also strongly rec­om­mended. Re­cent stud­ies have sug­gested that pel­leted diets are con­sid­ered nu­tri­tion­ally su­pe­rior to home­made diets and seed mix­tures and may re­duce the risk of ab­nor­mal grit con­sump­tion. (Athan and Deter, 2000; Faye, 2006)

Con­trib­u­tors

Tanya Dewey (ed­i­tor), An­i­mal Di­ver­sity Web.

Rachel Hol­man (au­thor), North­ern Michi­gan Uni­ver­sity, Alec R. Lind­say (ed­i­tor, in­struc­tor), North­ern Michi­gan Uni­ver­sity.

Glossary

Ethiopian

living in sub-Saharan Africa (south of 30 degrees north) and Madagascar.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

agricultural

living in landscapes dominated by human agriculture.

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

arboreal

Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

choruses

to jointly display, usually with sounds, at the same time as two or more other individuals of the same or different species

colonial

used loosely to describe any group of organisms living together or in close proximity to each other - for example nesting shorebirds that live in large colonies. More specifically refers to a group of organisms in which members act as specialized subunits (a continuous, modular society) - as in clonal organisms.

crepuscular

active at dawn and dusk

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
ecotourism

humans benefit economically by promoting tourism that focuses on the appreciation of natural areas or animals. Ecotourism implies that there are existing programs that profit from the appreciation of natural areas or animals.

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

forest

forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.

frugivore

an animal that mainly eats fruit

granivore

an animal that mainly eats seeds

herbivore

An animal that eats mainly plants or parts of plants.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

migratory

makes seasonal movements between breeding and wintering grounds

mimicry

imitates a communication signal or appearance of another kind of organism

monogamous

Having one mate at a time.

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

omnivore

an animal that mainly eats all kinds of things, including plants and animals

oviparous

reproduction in which eggs are released by the female; development of offspring occurs outside the mother's body.

pet trade

the business of buying and selling animals for people to keep in their homes as pets.

riparian

Referring to something living or located adjacent to a waterbody (usually, but not always, a river or stream).

scrub forest

scrub forests develop in areas that experience dry seasons.

seasonal breeding

breeding is confined to a particular season

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

suburban

living in residential areas on the outskirts of large cities or towns.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

territorial

defends an area within the home range, occupied by a single animals or group of animals of the same species and held through overt defense, display, or advertisement

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

tropical savanna and grassland

A terrestrial biome. Savannas are grasslands with scattered individual trees that do not form a closed canopy. Extensive savannas are found in parts of subtropical and tropical Africa and South America, and in Australia.

savanna

A grassland with scattered trees or scattered clumps of trees, a type of community intermediate between grassland and forest. See also Tropical savanna and grassland biome.

temperate grassland

A terrestrial biome found in temperate latitudes (>23.5° N or S latitude). Vegetation is made up mostly of grasses, the height and species diversity of which depend largely on the amount of moisture available. Fire and grazing are important in the long-term maintenance of grasslands.

visual

uses sight to communicate

Ref­er­ences

2006. "African Grey Par­rots" (On-line). Ac­cessed April 10, 2008 at Eliteparrotsclub.​com/​articles/​species/​mediumsmall/​africangreyparrots.​html.

2008. "CITES species data­base" (On-line). CITES. Ac­cessed April 14, 2008 at http://​www.​cites.​org/​eng/​resources/​species.​html.

2008. "Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus (African Grey Par­rot, Congo African Grey Par­rot, Grey Par­rot)" (On-line). Zipcodezoo.​com. Ac­cessed March 20, 2008 at http://​zipcodezoo.​com/​Animals/​P/​Psittacus_​erithacus.​asp.

2008. "Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus, Lin­naeeus, 1758" (On-line pdf). Ac­cessed March 20, 2008 at http://​www.​cites.​org/​eng/​com/​AC/​22/​E22-10-2-Al.​pdf.

2008. "The IUCN Red List of Threat­ened Species" (On-line). Ac­cessed April 15, 2008 at http://​www.​iuncredlist.​org/​search/​details.​php/​47991/​clss.

Athan, M. 1999. Bar­ron's Guide to Com­pan­ion Par­rot Be­hav­ior. New York, New York: Bar­ron's Ed­u­ca­tional Se­ries.

Athan, M., D. Deter. 2000. The African Grey Par­rot Hand­book. Haup­pauge, NY: Bar­ron's Ed­u­ca­tional Se­ries. Ac­cessed March 20, 2008 at http://​books.​google.​com/​books?​id=qqrxmrS2bXQC.

Bot­toni, L., R. Massa, D. Boero. 2003. The Grey Par­rot (Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus) as a Mu­si­cian: an Ex­per­i­ment With the Tem­per­ate Scale. Ethol­ogy Ecol­ogy and Evo­lu­tion, 15: 133-141.

Fahlman, A. 2002. "African Drey Par­rot Con­ser­va­tion: a Fea­si­bil­ity eval­u­a­tion of De­vel­op­ing a Local Con­ser­va­tion Pro­gram in Pricipe" (On-line pdf). Ac­cessed April 10, 2008 at http://​www.​env-impact.​geo.​uu.​se/​84Fahlman.​pdf.

Faye, S. 2006. "African Greys aka Grey Par­rots, Gen­eral Info on the African Grey" (On-line). Avian­Web:Pet Bird Re­sources. Ac­cessed April 10, 2008 at http://​www.​avianweb.​com/​aricangreys.​htm.

Galef, B. 2004. So­cial Learn­ing and Im­i­ta­tion. Bi­ol­ogy of Be­hav­ior, 4: 261-269.

Hal­lan­der, J. 2001. "Flock Be­hav­ior: How it Af­fects our Com­pan­ion Par­rots" (On-line). The Grey Play Round­table, African Grey Info. Ac­cessed April 10, 2008 at africangreys.​com/​behavior/​flocks.​htm.

Juste, J. 1995. Trade in the Gray Par­rot (Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus) on the Is­land of Principe (SAO TOME and Principe, Cen­tral Africa): Ini­tial As­sess­ment of the Ac­tiv­ity on Its Im­pact. Bi­o­log­i­cal Con­ser­va­tion, 76: 101-104.

Luescher, A. 2006. Man­ual of Par­rot Be­hav­ior. New York, New York: Black­well Pub­lish­ing.

Melo, M., C. O'Ryan. 2007. Ge­netic dif­fer­en­ti­a­tion be­tween Principe Is­land and main­land pop­u­la­tions of the grey par­rot (Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus), and im­pli­ca­tions for con­ser­va­tion. Mol­e­c­u­lar Ecol­ogy, 16: 1673-1685.

Pep­per­berg, I. 2000. The Alex Stud­ies, Cog­ni­tive and Com­mu­nica­tive Abil­i­ties of Grey Par­rots.b. Boston, Mass­a­chu­setts: Har­vard Uni­ver­sity Press.

Pep­per­berg, I. 1991. A Com­mu­nica­tive Ap­proach to An­i­mal Cog­ni­tion: A Study of Con­cep­tual Abil­i­ties of an African Grey Par­rot. Pp. 153-186 in C Ris­tau, ed. Cog­ni­tive Ethol­ogy, the Minds of Other An­i­mals, Es­says in Honor of Don­ald R. Grif­fin. Mah­wah, New Jer­sey: Lawrence Erl­baum As­so­ci­ates.

Pep­per­berg, I. 2007. When Train­ing En­gen­ders Fail­ure to Im­i­tate in Grey Par­rots (Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus). Pro­ceed­ings of the 4th In­ter­na­tional Sym­po­sium on Im­i­ta­tion in An­i­mals and Ar­ti­facts: 0-10.

Pep­per­berg, I. 2006. cog­ni­tive and Com­mu­nica­tive Abil­i­ties of Grey Par­rots. Ap­plied An­i­mal Be­hav­ior Sci­ence, 100: 77-86.

Pep­per­berg, I. 2001. "Lessons from Cog­ni­tive Ethol­ogy: An­i­mal Mod­els for Etho­log­i­cal Com­put­ing" (On-line pdf). Ac­cessed April 10, 2008 at http://​www.​lehigh.​edu/​~mhbo/cogsZwe­bread­ings/Pepperberg23sep01.​pdf.

Ryan, T. 2002. Grit Im­paction in Two Neona­tal African Grey Par­rots (Psit­ta­cus eritha­cus eritha­cus). Avian Med­i­cine and Surgery, 16: 230-233.

Wright, M. 2002. "Un­der­stand­ing the Wild Na­ture of our Dreys" (On-line). Ac­cessed April 10, 2008 at http://​www.​lafeber.​com/​Laferber-Library/​Articles/​wright/​wildnature.​asp.