Geographic Range
Papio cynocephalus is a broadly distributed species, ranging through most of Eastern Africa south of the equatorial rain forests. These baboons occur in Botswana, Kenya, Malawi, Somalia, Tanzania, Zaire, and Zimbabwe. They are found east of the Luangwa in Zambia, in Northern Mozambique and throughout most of Tanzania.
This species is part of a complex of closely related African baboon species. We have
an account of the whole genus under
Papio
.
Habitat
Yellow baboons are found in savannah, grassland steppe, and rainforest habitats.
- Habitat Regions
- tropical
- Terrestrial Biomes
- savanna or grassland
- chaparral
- forest
- scrub forest
- Other Habitat Features
- agricultural
Physical Description
These monkeys are highly sexually dimorphic. Males weigh around 23 kg and females
around 12 kg. The head and body length ranges between 508 and 1,143 mm, with the tail
adding an additional 456 to 711 mm to the total length. These animals have 32 teeth.
The first lower premolar is modified and serves as a hone for the upper canine tooth.
Males have large canine teeth, whereas the teeth of females are much smaller. There
is significant geographic variation in average body size and skull size, as well
as in the texture of pelage. The three forms most often described are Ibean baboons,
Kinda baboons, and "typical" yellow baboons.
The orientation of the face in yellow baboons is normal, helping to distinguish them
from
Papio ursinus
which has a downwardly flexed face. The post-orbital constriction of these animals
is less pronounced than in
anubis
and
chacma
baboons. Ibean yellow baboons have large skulls. Typical yellow baboons have medium
to large skulls. Kinda baboons are noted for their greatly reduced cranial size,
associated smaller teeth, and weakly expressed temporal lines.
Pelage is characteristically a yellowish-brown. This overall color is produced by
individual hairs which are yellow-brown for most of their length but have a black
tips. In typical yellow baboons, both males and females are unmaned. However, in
the Ibean form of this species, males have a weakly expressed mane. It is not at
all comparable to the large mane found in
Papio hamadryas
,
Papio anubis
, and
Papio papio
. The head has a prominent crest on the top, produced by the longish hairs which
grow upward toward the crest. This helps to distinguish this species from
P. anubis
which has a "flat" head. The pelage of typical and Kinda forms of this species is
straight. The hair of Kinda baboons is reported to be very silky in texture. In
contrast, the hairs of Ibean baboons are wavey and coarser than those of the other
yellow baboons. All yellow baboons have hands and feet that are the same color as
the rest of the body, and silver-colored fringes on the hands and feet.
The skin of the face and around the ischial callosities of
P. cynocephalus
is purplish-black in both sexes. The bare area of the rump is much smaller in this
species than in
P. hamadryas
or
P. papio
.
Tail shape varies geographically. The tail of Ibean baboons resembles that of
P. anubis
, with the proximal portion extending straight out from the rump, and the distal 3/4
falling limp, as if the tail has been broken. Kinda baboons have a gracefully arched
tail. Typical yellow baboons generally have the bent phenotype, but are variable,
with some individuals showing the arched tail morphology.
In Ibean and typical yellow baboons, the natal pelage is black. This fur is replaced
by the typical yellowish-brown by about 6 months of age. In sharp contrast, the natal
coat of Kinda baboons is a reddish-brown color.
Yellow baboons have a somewhat "pointed" nose, and are thinner through the chest than
are
anubis baboons
.
- Other Physical Features
- endothermic
- homoiothermic
- bilateral symmetry
- Sexual Dimorphism
- male larger
- ornamentation
Reproduction
Reproductive behavior in
P. cynocephalus
is closely tied to social organization. Because these animals live in multi-male,
multi-female troops, there is the potential for any male to mate with any female.
Males therefore compete fiercely for access to sexually receptive females. In general,
a male’s ability to consort with a female and exclude other males from access to her
is related to the male’s ability to compete with other males. There is therefore
a correlation between male dominance rank within the troop and mating success. Larger,
younger, and stronger males have a distinct advantage in this type of competition.
It should be noted that as in many other social animals, there can be other factors
which affect a male’s mating success. For example, males may form alliances with
other males, subverting the normal dominance hierarchy. Two males, neither of whom
can dominate a third male alone, may join forces. As a coalition, these males may
succeed in securing access to a sexually receptive female. Such coalitions are reciprocal,
and typically occur between pairs of older males who are well acquainted with one
another through mutual tenure in a troop.
Males also follow a strategy of developing "friendships" with females This enhances
their opportunities to mate. In these friendships, males groom, share food, and have
strong affiliative ties with particular females and their offspring. It is common
for males to defend their female friends during agonistic encounters with other females,
and with other males. These associations are not confined to the period during which
females are sexually receptive, but span the entire gamut of the female’s reproductive
life—including pregnancy, lactation, and time spent cycling. Females exhibit a preference
for mating with their male friends, and therefore make consortships with their male
friends more likely. In addition, because females prefer their friends as mates,
they are more likely to cooperate with them in the maintenance of a consortship than
they are to cooperate with other, less favored, males.
In
P. cynocephalus
, females mate with a variety of males during any given estrus cycle. Such multiple
matings are not necessary to ensure fertilization, and may function to confuse the
actual paternity of the female’s offspring. This may help to mitigate infanticidal
tendencies of males.
- Mating System
- polygynandrous (promiscuous)
Reproduction in
P. cynocephalus
is related to the social structure of this species. Yellow baboons live in multi-male,
multi-female troops. Mating is polygynandrous, with both males and females mating
with multiple partners. Most matings occur during consortships. Consortships arise
when a male, through aggression toward potential rivals, is able to maintain exclusive
sexual access to a female. Females may consort with multiple males while they are
sexually receptive, although they consort with only one male at a time. Because
it is apparently easier for a male to maintain exclusive access to a female if the
female is cooperative, there is a significant amount of female mate choice, with females
preferring some partners over others.
Females characteristically have an estrous cycle of 32 days in length, however, cycle
length varies between individuals and between populations. At the Tana River National
Primate Reserve in Kenya, primiparous females had the longest cycles, averaging 44
days, and multiparous females had the shortest cycles, averaging 37 days. These cycles
are much longer than the 32 to 34 day cycles reported in Amboseli National Park.
Differences may be related to nutritional differences between populations, social
differences, or they may reflect underlying genetic differences.
There is a noticeable menstrual flow for approximately three days per cycle if the
female does not conceive. During the period around ovulation, the perineal skin
of the female swells, alerting the males to her potentially fertile condition, and
enhancing her attractiveness to them. Mating is initiated by the female, who presents
her hindquarters to the male. When females are cycling, mating frequency can be from
1 to 6 times per hour. When females become pregnant, they cease to mate at all.
Their pericallosal skin turns red as a result of increased blood flow to the region,
alerting other members of the group to their reproductive condition.
Gestation lasts about 175 to 181 days, after which the female gives birth to a single offspring, weighing approximately 854 g. This is significantly smaller than the 1068 g neonates reported for P. anubis . The neonate has a black or reddish coat, depending upon the subspecies. This makes it easy to distinguish neonates from older infants. An infant is completely dependent upon its mother for the first few months, until it begins to eat solid food and is able to walk on its own. Age at independence is difficult to estimate, because even if the mother dies, a young baboon may continue to receive care from adult males, or other female kin. Independence is often listed as the age of weaning.
Females have an interbirth interval of approximately 21 to 27 months. This interval
varies geographically, as well as according to maternal age and maternal rank. Interbirth
intervals were longer at Tana River than at Amboseli, perhaps in part because the
probability of infant survival to 24 months of age was greater. However, in
anubis baboons
, it has been shown that females who are older or have a higher rank tend to have
shorter interbirth intervals, probably because these females experience enhanced nutritional
status and are free from harassment by higher ranking females. The interbirth interval
is also shorter if an infant dies before weaning.
Weaning occurs sometime around 1 year of age. Lactation is a huge cost for adult
females, and has been shown to cause a reduction in female weight. Lower ranking
and younger females probably take longer to recover adequate body weight to reproduce
than do older, dominant females, explaining their longer interbirth intervals.
The onset of puberty and attainment of adult size is highly variable and is associated
with nutrition levels in closely related
anubis baboons
. The effect of nutrition on growth is so strong that as little as 15 to 16 weeks
of dietary variation in newborns can have lasting effects on overall rates of female
growth, absolute adult weight, and age at menarche. This may be one reason that differences
have been noted in the age of menarche in geographically separated populations.
In yellow baboons, puberty occurs between the ages of 5 and 6 years in females, and
is signaled by menarche, or in some cases, first pregnancy. In males, there is greater
variation in age at onset of puberty, with sexual maturation occurring between the
ages of 4 and 7 years. Between 70 and 97 per cent of males emigrate from their natal
troop sometime before reaching sexual maturity. Although females typically spend
their entire lives in their natal troops, some transfer of females to new groups has
been observed.
- Key Reproductive Features
- iteroparous
- year-round breeding
- gonochoric/gonochoristic/dioecious (sexes separate)
- sexual
- fertilization
- viviparous
Most parental behavior is performed by the female. Females nurse, groom, and play
with their offspring. Females express different patterns of infant care, often associated
with rank and age. In yellow baboons, higher-ranking females tend to be more "permissive"
in their parenting than lower ranking females, who tend to be more nervous and "restrictive,"
preventing their offspring from moving away from them. Such differences may be related
to the amount of harassment females of lower rank are likely to receive. Another difference
seen in maternal behavior in this species is that older mothers are known to spend
more time in contact or close to their infants and are less likely to terminate bouts
of nursing than are younger females. First-time mothers are also likely to reject
infants sooner than are experienced mothers. These differences may affect interbirth
intervals.
Cooperative care of offspring is not present in
P. cynocephalus
, but it is not uncommon for females other than the mother to groom an infant, sometimes
providing allomaternal care to the infant. Subadult and juvenile females who have
not yet reproduced themselves are most likely to exhibit allomaternal behavior. Other
factors known to affect the incidence of allomaternal behavior in most cercopithecine
species include the infant’s age, and relatedness of the allomother to the mother
and infant. As is the case for all baboons, infants are very attractive to other
members of the social group, and are the focus of a great deal of investigation and
attention, especially while they are still displaying their black natal coats. In
extreme cases, females may kidnap the offspring of other females. Lower-ranking females
have been reported to be subjected to this extreme form of harassment more often than
have higher-ranking females. However, in an analysis of infant handling, Bentley-Condit
et al. (2001) found no consistent association between mother’s rank and how often
her infant was handled. Although females attempted to handle infants of females of
like rank or lower more often than they attempted to handle the offspring of higher
ranking females, mothers were able to successfully rebuff more than three quarters
of all handling attempts.
Males have complex relationships with infants and juveniles, which in some cases may
be a form of parental care. Males are known to carry, protect, share food (especially
meat), groom, and play with, the offspring of their female friends. Because they
are more likely to mate with their female friends than they are with other females,
these infants and juveniles are more likely to be their own offspring than are other
immature animals within the troop. This behavior, therefore, can be interpreted as
paternal.
However, it should be noted that the relationship between adult males and these immature
animals may be more complex than this. There may be some form of reciprocity involved.
Adult males will often carry infants during tense interactions with other adult males.
This carrying can be initiated either by the adult male or by the infant. Such contact
with an infant during agonistic encounters may have the effect of inhibiting aggression
by other males. The favors bestowed upon an infant used as a buffer may therefore
be a form a "payback" from the adult male. However, since the tendency to use an
infant as an agonistic buffer is related to familiarity with the infant and the probability
of paternity, it is impossible to separate the nepotism from the reciprocity of such
interactions.
- Parental Investment
- altricial
-
pre-fertilization
- provisioning
-
protecting
- female
-
pre-hatching/birth
-
provisioning
- female
-
protecting
- male
- female
-
provisioning
-
pre-weaning/fledging
-
provisioning
- male
- female
-
protecting
- male
- female
-
provisioning
-
pre-independence
-
provisioning
- male
- female
-
protecting
- male
- female
-
provisioning
- post-independence association with parents
- extended period of juvenile learning
- inherits maternal/paternal territory
- maternal position in the dominance hierarchy affects status of young
Lifespan/Longevity
The maximum lifespan of a captive
hamadryas baboon
is measured at 37.5 years. A captive
chacma baboon
is reported to have lived 45 years in captivity. Although the lifespan of
P. cynocephalus
has not been reported, it is likely to be similar to these two species, although
somewhat shorter than these in the wild.
Behavior
Yellow baboons are quadrupedal, mainly terrestrial primates. They are highly social
animals, with a complex multi-male, multi-female social structure. Members of a
troop travel, forage, and sleep together. An average troop may be comprised of 20
to 180 animals. Home ranges averaging 2,408 ha have been reported. The daily range
of a troop averages 5,900 m. The movements of a troop may be limited by the availability
of appropriate sleeping locations. Because the troop beds down in trees, or on rocks/cliffs,
activity of the troop must be coordinated so that one of a set number of sleeping
sites can be reached by nightfall.
Males typically emigrate from their natal troops, with 70 to 95 per cent of males
emigrating prior to the time that they reach full adult size. Upon settling in a
new troop, males must establish themselves in the male dominance hierarchy of that
troop. This usually involves aggressive behavior between males, with the "winner"
of an encounter establishing dominance over the "loser." Immigrant males may receive
the most aggression from males of similar age, as is the case for anubis baboons.
For this reason,
anubis baboon
males have been reported to choose to immigrate into troops containing the fewest
like-aged males, and the same factor may play a role when yellow baboon males are
choosing a troop in which to settle.
Baboon males apparently leave their natal groups of their own accord. They are not
expelled from their natal troop, as some have thought. Emigration of males may be
related to sexual attraction to unfamiliar females. Male baboons entering a new troop
may direct most of their energy to interacting with females, competing for greater
access to estrous females than they had in their natal group. Concomitantly, female
baboons apparently prefer to mate with unfamiliar males, often soliciting copulation
from new immigrant males. Because of their dispersal pattern, males of
P. cynocephalus
do not maintain long-term bonds with their male kin, as is seen in
hamadryas baboons
.
The timing of male emigration can vary, and it may be associated with genetic differences
between animals. At Amboseli National Park in Kenya, male
P. cynocephalus
typically leave their natal group around the time that they reach full adult size.
However, males the in the closely related species
P. anubis
, tend to emigrate as juveniles or subadults. Interestingly, where hybridization
between these two species occurs, the hybrids tend to emigrate according to the pattern
expressed by the species they most resemble. Males with strongly "anubis" phenotypes
tend to emigrate earlier than do hybrid males with strongly "yellow" phenotypes.
The reasons for this difference, genetic or social, are not known.
Whether or not a male is accepted in a group depends greatly on how the females in
that troop respond to him. If the females, for whatever reason, do not support the
male, and do not wish to mate with him, his transfer into their group may be a lengthy,
difficult, and unrewarding process. Females can exhibit some degree of mate choice
by accepting or denying males who wish to immigrate into their troop.
Because males do not maintain life-long social ties with their kin, it is female
kinship that forms the core and stability of
P. cynocephalus
society. Because females of this species do not emigrate from their natal groups,
female kin have life-long associations. Within a troop of yellow baboons, there is
a dominance hierarchy of matrilines, or female lineages, which is very stable over
time. Generally, an individual female occupies a place in the dominance hierarchy
immediately below her mother and her younger sisters. Dominance relationships appear
to develop from infancy, when maternal kin intervene in encounters with other baboons,
and through the differential treatment of the young of higher-ranking females by unrelated
animals. Within a matriline, the dominance relationships of sisters are the inverse
of birth order.
The dominance rank of females within a troop may affect a number of different aspects
of the female’s life. For example, animals with higher dominance rank may have greater
access to preferred food resources. This, in turn, may affect their ability to produce
healthy offspring, survivorship of young, or the frequency with which they can produce
offspring. In addition to having important reproductive consequences, high dominance
rank may protect a female from harassment, especially when she has an infant.
The female dominance hierarchy is not continually challenged by the arrival of new
females in the troop, which probably allows for the stability of female dominance
relationships over time. Perhaps because relatedness of females within troops is
inherently higher than the relatedness of females between troops, female baboons are
likely to take the lead in aggressive encounters with other troops of baboons. Females
are apparently acting to defend their food resources, and access to those resources
in the future, during such encounters.
Female kin behave differently toward one another than do unrelated animals. Females
are more likely to aid their kin during disputes than they are to become involved
in disputes of unrelated individuals. These relationship can extend over multiple
generations, with females aiding not only their own offspring, and aunts aiding nieces,
but grandmothers aiding their granddaughters.
Because female
P. cynocephalus
are long-lived and philopatric, they are able to develop very complex social relationships.
Getting and keeping the right friends in yellow baboon society can have strong implications
for maintenance of rank, as well as keeping or losing one’s fetus. One might therefore
expect that female baboons would maintain their closest ties with females of similar
rank. However, this does not appear to be the case. Rank was not a good predictor
of preferred partners for the females at the Tana River National Primate Reserve in
Kenya.
Within socially similar cercopithecines, females have been known to establish relationships
across matrilines. Females may befriend others who are of similar rank. Also, low
ranking females may attempt to gain favor with higher ranking females by providing
grooming and other affiliate behaviors. An ally with higher rank may provide a low
ranking female with preferential access to scarce food resources, or support during
some aggressive encounters.
- Key Behaviors
- terricolous
- diurnal
- motile
- sedentary
- territorial
- social
- dominance hierarchies
Home Range
Home ranges averaging 2,408 ha have been reported. The daily range of a troop averages
5,900 m.
Communication and Perception
As in all highly social species, communication is varied and complex. Yellow baboons
utilize visual signals and gestures, vocalizations, and tactile communication.
Visual signals include
social presenting
, in which a female or juvenile displays its hind quarters to a male. It can also
be done by a female who has approached another female with her black infant. This
submissive signal differs from sexual presenting (which females do to elicit copulation),
and is often accompanied by lip smacking.
Staring
is a threat behavior, the effect of which is enhanced by the differently colored
fur in the region of the eye which is revealed when the baboon stares.
Eyebrow raising
reveals the lighter fur in the area of the eyelid, and is an aggressive gesture.
Canine tooth display
through a
tension yawn
is another threatening gesture. It can be directed toward a rival male, a predator,
or is performed by lower-raking males toward higher-ranking males who are consorting
with estrus females or who possess meat. Male baboons who are close to one another
can use
tooth grinding
to threaten one another. Baboons retreating from high-tension situations use
rapid glances
to break tension. An adult male who is guarding sometimes sit with his erect penis
is plain view. This
penile display
communicates the male’s presence to other males.
Teeth chattering
and
lipsmacking
, although not technically vocalizations, are auditory cues of reassurance, often
performed by a dominant animal when another is presenting to him.
Vocalizations made by yellow baboons include a
two-phase bark
, or "wahoo" call, which adult males direct toward feline predators or toward other
males. It is thought to communicate the presence of the male and his arousal. Adult
male yellow baboons make
grunting
vocalizations as a threat.
Screeching
is common during aggressive encounters, and can be made by any age or sex class.
Subadult and adult yellow baboons produce a
yakking
call when retreating from a threatening animal. This call is often accompanied
by a grimace of fear. A
shrill bark
is produced by all except adult males to indicate alarm, especially due to sudden
disturbances.
Rhythmic grunting
may be produced by all yellow baboons except infants when they wish to signal amicable
intentions to another animal. Finally, adult and subadult yellow baboons of both
sexes are known to produce a
dog-like bark
when they become separated from the main part of the troop.
Tactile communciation is common in cercopithecines.
Social grooming
is used to reinforce social bonds, as well as to remove parasites and debris from
the fur.
Social mounting
is a reassurance behavior.
Anubis baboons
also perform a friendly
nose-to-nose
greeting.
Chemical communication has not been reported for yellow baboons. However, female
anubis baboons
are known to produce aliphatic acids when they are sexually receptive. These acids
are thought to enhance a female’s sexual attractiveness. Because there is considerable
hybridization between anubis baboons and yellow baboons, and because the Ibean form
of yellow baboons may have arizen originally through hybridization between typical
yellow baboons and
anubis baboons
, it would be interesting to examine various yellow baboon populations for production
of aliphatic acids.
Food Habits
Although generally described as frugivorous, yellow baboons are dietary generalists.
they are known to eat pods, grass, sedges, seeds, fruit, roots, leaves, buds, bark,
flowers, insects, and meat. They are known hunt and kill rabbits and
vervet monkeys
.
One feeding adaptation thought to be shared by all baboons is the ability to subsist
on a relatively low quality diet. Baboons can subsist on grasses for extended periods
of time. This allows them to exploit dry terrestrial habitats, like deserts, semideserts,
steppes, and grasslands.
The feeding behavior of yellow baboons has been heavily studied, especially as it
impacts survival of immature animals. Immatures born late in the wet season when
the number of foods and food parts eaten is highest have the highest survival to four
years of age. This makes sense, as there is ample food for the lactating mother.
Survival of immatures is lowest for those born late in the dry season when the number
of foods and food parts eaten is the lowest. Survival to two years closely parallels
the feeding curves.
- Primary Diet
- omnivore
- Animal Foods
- mammals
- carrion
- insects
- Plant Foods
- leaves
- roots and tubers
- seeds, grains, and nuts
- fruit
Predation
Three predator species are known for yellow baboons. The predation rate varies by
population, and is estimated at between 4 and 8 percent per year. Of those who fall
victim to predation, about 40% are infants, 30% are juveniles, and 13% are adult females.
The remainder are adult males.
Although yellow baboons typically flee when faced with a potential predator, they
have also been reported to respond aggressively to potential predators. They have
been observed killing domestic dogs, mobbing smaller carnivores, such as jackals and
cheetahs, and have even fought leopards and lions.
There may be a relationship between where in the troop different age or sex classes
of baboons travel as it relates to risk of death from predators. However, research
on this matter has been largely equivocal.
Ecosystem Roles
As predators, yellow baboons may affect the populations of prey items. As prey, they
may support predator populations. Baboons may also help to disperse seeds, by passing
some through their bodies undigested, or carrying fruits away from trees. They undoubtedly
aid in soil aeration from digging for roots and tubers.
- Ecosystem Impact
- disperses seeds
- soil aeration
Economic Importance for Humans: Positive
Baboons are used heavily in biomedical and behavioral research. They are entertaining
creatures enjoyed by zoo visitors and ecotourists.
- Positive Impacts
- ecotourism
- research and education
Economic Importance for Humans: Negative
Baboons are not shy creatures, and where they come into contact with humans, they
can be problematic. They are known to raid crops, and can bite humans if approached
too closely.
- Negative Impacts
-
injures humans
- bites or stings
- crop pest
Conservation Status
Papio cynocepahlus is not thought to be especially threatened at this time. However, as is true of all primates, it is listed on CITES appendix II.
Other Comments
Anubis baboons (
P. anubis
) are known to hybridize freely in the wild with yellow baboons,
P. cynocephalus
. In the Amboseli national park in Kenya, the amount of reported hybridization
between these two species has increased over time. Researchers think that the increasing
immigration of anubis males into yellow baboon troops is responsible for the increase
noted in hybrid characters. Because the slopes of Mount Kilamanjaro are under increasing
agricultural pressure, it is likely that anubis males have no alternative areas into
which to emigrate.
The only differences noted in behavior of hybrid animals is that males with anubis-like
features (e.g. coarser hair, longer manes, darker coloration, broader chests, and
more sharply "broken" tails) tend to emigrate from their natal group as juveniles
or subadults, rather than as full adults. This behavior has been seen occasionally
in the anubis baboons of the Gombe preserve in Tanzania, but is not known in yellow
baboons.
The hybridization between anubis and yellow baboons seems to have a long history.
Interestingly, the Ibean form of yellow baboon, which has coarser hair than typical
yellow baboons, a more pronounced mane, and other somewhat "anubis-like" features,
is thought by some researchers to be evidence of the historical influx of anubis genes
into yellow baboon populations.
Additional Links
Contributors
Nancy Shefferly (author), Animal Diversity Web.
- Ethiopian
-
living in sub-Saharan Africa (south of 30 degrees north) and Madagascar.
- native range
-
the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.
- tropical
-
the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.
- tropical savanna and grassland
-
A terrestrial biome. Savannas are grasslands with scattered individual trees that do not form a closed canopy. Extensive savannas are found in parts of subtropical and tropical Africa and South America, and in Australia.
- savanna
-
A grassland with scattered trees or scattered clumps of trees, a type of community intermediate between grassland and forest. See also Tropical savanna and grassland biome.
- temperate grassland
-
A terrestrial biome found in temperate latitudes (>23.5° N or S latitude). Vegetation is made up mostly of grasses, the height and species diversity of which depend largely on the amount of moisture available. Fire and grazing are important in the long-term maintenance of grasslands.
- chaparral
-
Found in coastal areas between 30 and 40 degrees latitude, in areas with a Mediterranean climate. Vegetation is dominated by stands of dense, spiny shrubs with tough (hard or waxy) evergreen leaves. May be maintained by periodic fire. In South America it includes the scrub ecotone between forest and paramo.
- forest
-
forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.
- scrub forest
-
scrub forests develop in areas that experience dry seasons.
- agricultural
-
living in landscapes dominated by human agriculture.
- endothermic
-
animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.
- bilateral symmetry
-
having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.
- sexual ornamentation
-
one of the sexes (usually males) has special physical structures used in courting the other sex or fighting the same sex. For example: antlers, elongated tails, special spurs.
- polygynandrous
-
the kind of polygamy in which a female pairs with several males, each of which also pairs with several different females.
- iteroparous
-
offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).
- year-round breeding
-
breeding takes place throughout the year
- sexual
-
reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female
- fertilization
-
union of egg and spermatozoan
- viviparous
-
reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.
- altricial
-
young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.
- diurnal
-
- active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
- motile
-
having the capacity to move from one place to another.
- sedentary
-
remains in the same area
- territorial
-
defends an area within the home range, occupied by a single animals or group of animals of the same species and held through overt defense, display, or advertisement
- social
-
associates with others of its species; forms social groups.
- dominance hierarchies
-
ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates
- visual
-
uses sight to communicate
- tactile
-
uses touch to communicate
- acoustic
-
uses sound to communicate
- visual
-
uses sight to communicate
- tactile
-
uses touch to communicate
- acoustic
-
uses sound to communicate
- chemical
-
uses smells or other chemicals to communicate
- carrion
-
flesh of dead animals.
- soil aeration
-
digs and breaks up soil so air and water can get in
- ecotourism
-
humans benefit economically by promoting tourism that focuses on the appreciation of natural areas or animals. Ecotourism implies that there are existing programs that profit from the appreciation of natural areas or animals.
- omnivore
-
an animal that mainly eats all kinds of things, including plants and animals
References
Alberts, S., J. Altmann. 2001. Immigration and hybridization patterns of yellow and anubis baboons in and around Amboseli, Kenya. American Journal of Primatology , `53: 139-154.
Altmann, J. 1980. Baboon Mothers and Infants . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Bentley-Condit, V., T. Moore, E. Smith. 2001. Analysis of infant handling and the effects of female rank among Tana River adult female yellow baboons ( Papio cynocephalus cynocepahlys ) using permutation/randomization tests. American Journal of Primatology , 55: 117-130.
Bentley-Condit, V., E. Smith. 1999. Female dominance and female social relationships among yellow baboons ( Papio hamadryas cynocephalus ). American Journal of Primatology , 47: 321-334.
Bentley-Condit, V., E. Smith. 1997. Female reproductive parameters of Tana River yellow baboons. International Journal of Primatology , 18/4: 581-595.
Bercovitch, F. 1987. Female weight and reproductive condition in a population of olive baboons ( Papio anubis ). American Journal of Primatology , 12: 189-195.
Cheney, D., R. Wrangham. 1987. Predation. Pp. 227-239 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of chicago Press.
Disotell, T. 2000. Molecular systematics of the Cercopithicidae. Pp. 29-56 in Old World Monkeys . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Dunbar, R. 1987. Demography and reproduction. Pp. 240-249 in Primate Socieites . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Eldredge, N. 1993. What, if anything, is a species?. Species, Species Concepts, and Primate Evolution . London: Plenum Press.
Groves, C. 2001. Primate Taxonomy . Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Insitution Press.
Groves, C. 2000. The Phylogeny of the Cercopithecoides. Pp. 77-100 in Old World Monkeys . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PRess.
Hamilton III, W., J. Bulger. 1992. Facultative expresion of behavioral differences between one-male and multimale savanna baboon groups. American Journal of Primatology , 28: 61-71.
Harvey, P., R. Martin, T. Clutton-Brock. 1987. Life histories in comparative perspective. Pp. 181-196 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Hrdy, S., P. Whitten. 1987. Patterning of sexual activity. Pp. 370-384 in Primate Socieities . Chicago: The University of Chicago PRess.
Jolly, C., P. Whitehead. 2000. Old world monkeys: Three decades of development and change in the study of the Cercopithicoidea. Pp. 1-28 in Old World Monkeys . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Jolly, C. 1993. Species, subspecies, and baboon systematics. Pp. 67-107 in Species, Species Concepts, and Primate Evolution . New York: Plenum Publishing.
Jolly, C., J. Phillips-Conroy. 2003. Testicular size, mating system, and maturation schedules in wild anubis and hamadryas baboons. International Journal of Primatology , 24/1: 125-142.
Kummer, H. 1968. Social Organisation of Hamdryas Baboons. A Field Study . Basel and Chicago: Karger, and University Press.
Masters, J. 1993. Primates and paradigms: Problems with the identification of genetic species. Pp. 43-64 in Species, Species Concepts, and Primate Evolution . New York: Plenum Publishing.
Mayr, E. 1942. Systematics and the Origin of Species . New York: Columbia University Press.
Melnick, D., M. Pearl. 1987. Cercopithecines in multimale groups: Genetic diversity and population structure. Pp. 121-134 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Napier, J., P. Napier. 1985. The natural history of the primates . Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.
Nicolson, N. 1987. Infants, mothers, and other females. Pp. 330-342 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Nowak, R. 1999. Walker's Mammals of the World, Sixth Edition . Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Oates, J. 1987. Food distribution and foraging behavior. Pp. 197-209 in Primat Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Phillips-Conroy, J., C. Jolly, P. Nystrom, H. Hemmalin. 1992. Migration of male hamadryas baboons into anubis groups in the Awash National Park, Ethiopia. International Journal of Primatology , 12/4: 455-475.
Primate Info Net, 2000. "Primate Info Net" (On-line). Yellow Baboon ( Papio cynocephalus ). Accessed July 14, 2003 at http://www.primate.wisc.edu/pin/factsheets/papio_cynocephalus.html .
Pusey, A., C. Packer. 1987. Dispersal and philopatry. Pp. 250-266 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Rhine, R., G. Norton, J. Rogers, S. Wasser. 1992. Secondary sex ratio and maternal dominance rank among wild yellow baboons ( Papio cynocephalus ) of Mikumi National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology , 27: 261-273.
Rhine, R., G. Norton, G. Wynn, . Wynn. 1989. Plant feeding of yellow baboons ( Papio cynocephalus ) in Mikumi National Park, Tanzania, and the relationship between seasonal feeding and immature survival. International Journal of Primatology , 10/4: 319-341.
Rhine, R., R. Tilson. 1987. Reactions to fear as a proximate factor in the sociospatial organization of baboon progressions. American Journal of Primatology , 13: 119-128.
Rhine, R., S. Wasser, G. Norton. 1988. Eight-year study of social and ecological correlates of mortality among immature baboons of Mikumi National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology , 16: 199-212.
Rogers, J. 2000. Molecular genetic variation and population structure in Papio baboons. Pp. 57-76 in Old World Monkeys . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Sapolsky, R., J. Ray. 1989. Styles of dominance and their endocrine correlates among wild olive baboons ( Papio anubis ). American Journal of Primatology , 18: 1-13.
Sapolsky, R. 1996. Why should an aged male baboon ever transfer troops?. American Journal of Primatology , 39. Accessed (Date Unknown) at 149-157 .
Smuts, B., N. Nicolson. 1989. Reproduction in wild female olive baboons. American Journal of Primatology , 19: 229-246.
Smuts, B. 1987. Gender, aggression, and influence. Pp. 400-412 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Smuts, B. 1987. Sexual competition and mate choice. Pp. 385-399 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Stammbach, E. 1987. Desert, forest, and montain baboons: Multilevel societies. Pp. 112-120 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Stein, D. 1984. The Sociobiology of Adult Male and Infant Baboons . Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Strum, S. 1991. Weight and age in wild olive baboons. American Journal of Primatology , 25: 219-237.
Swedell, L. 2002. Affiliation among females in wild hamadryas baboons ( Papio hamadryas hamadryas ). International Journal of Primatology , 23/6: 1205-1225.
Szalay, F. 1993. Species concepts: The tested, the untestable, and the redundant. Pp. 27-41 in Species, Species Concepts, and Primate Evolution . New York: Plenum Press.
Thorington, R., C. Groves. 1970. An annotated classification of the Cercopithecoidea. Pp. 629-647 in Old World Moneys . New York: Academic Press.
Virgin, Jr., C., R. Sapolsky. 1997. Styles of male social behavior and their endocrine correlates among low-ranking male baboons. American Journal of Primatology , 42: 25-39.
Walters, J. 1987. Transition to Adulthood. Pp. 358-369 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Walters, R., R. Seyfarth. 1987. Conflict and cooperation. Pp. 306-317 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Whitten, P. 1987. Infants and adult males. Pp. 343-357 in Primate Societies . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Williams-Blangero, S., J. Vandenberg, J. Blangero, L. Konigsberg, B. Dyke. 1990. Genetic differentiation between baboon subspecies: Relevance for biomedical research. American Journal of Primatology , 20: 67-81.
Zinner, D., F. Pelaez, F. Torkler. 2001. Group composition and adult sex-ratio of hamadryas baoons ( Papio hamadryas hamadryas ) in central Eritrea. International Journal of Primatology , 22/3: 415-429.
Zinner, D., F. Pelaez. 1999. Verreaux's eagles ( Aquila verreauxi ) as potential predators of hamadryas baboons ( Papio hamadryas hamadryas ). American Journal of Primatology , 47: 61-66.
Zinner, D., T. Deschner. 2000. Sexual swellings in female hamadryas baboons after male take-overs: "Deceptive" swellings as a possible female counter-strategy against infanticideticide. American Journal of Primatology , 52: 157-168.