Pongoorangutan

Di­ver­sity

Pongo spp con­sists of three species of orang­utan, the largest ar­bo­real species, and a mem­ber of the Great Apes. The first two ar­tic­u­lated species were P. abelii (Les­son 1827)(Suma­tran Orang­utan) and P. pyg­maeus (Lin­neus 1760) (Bornean Orang­utan) who were rec­og­nized as dis­tinct species since 2001 based on mi­to­chon­dr­ial DNA analy­sis. How­ever, a small south­ern pop­u­la­tion of the Suma­tran Orang­utan, pre­vi­ously thought of as P. abelii, was rec­og­nized as their own species in 2017 as P. tapan­ulien­sis (Tapan­uli orang­utan). This was due to a com­bi­na­tion of small mor­pho­log­i­cal dif­fer­ences like skull size, first molar size, hair tex­ture and ge­nomic di­ver­sity. The mor­pho­log­i­cal and be­ha­vo­r­ial dif­fer­ences be­tween P. abelii and P. pyg­maeus are more ap­par­ent, with Bornean orang­utans (P. pyg­maeus) com­ing to the ground less fre­quently than P. abelii and hav­ing more fatty flanges in the adult males. ("Mor­pho­me­t­ric, Be­hav­ioral, and Ge­nomic Ev­i­dence for a New Orang­utan Species", 2017; Xu and Ar­na­son, 1996)

Ge­o­graphic Range

All three species of genus Pongo are found in the Ori­en­tal Re­gion, specif­i­cally the is­lands of In­done­sia. P. abelii and P. tapan­ulien­sis are found in North­ern Suma­tra. P. pyg­maeus is found ex­clu­sively in Bor­neo, from the north­ern provinces of Sabah to Kali­man­tan. ("Bornean Orang­utan", 08 Feb­ru­ary 2016; "Suma­tran Orang­utan", 16 Oc­to­ber 2017; "Tapan­uli Orang­utan", 11 Oc­to­ber 2017)

  • Biogeographic Regions
  • indian ocean

Habi­tat

Mem­bers of Pongo spp. re­side mainly in trop­i­cal ever­green forests, but their high­est pop­u­la­tion den­si­ties occur in swamps, fer­tile val­leys, and low­land soils. Orang­utans pre­fer places with the most fer­tile soil, as this will yield the most fruit. Fruit trees in the Meli­acedae (ma­hogany) fam­ily rep­re­sent a com­mon fea­ture of many of their cho­sen habi­tats. Gen­er­ally, it is as­sumed that Bornean orang­utans have smaller ranges than those on Suma­tra, but this is not the case. Males typ­i­cally have larger ranges than fe­males, and both male and fe­male ranges will over­lap with other orang­utans. Range size is de­bated and varies from 850ha. to 2500ha, and per­haps even more.

Orang­utans also uti­lize dis­turbed and frag­mented forests, a trait that is price­less with the rapid de­te­ri­o­ra­tion of their old-growth habi­tat. They pre­fer areas of in­creased canopy clo­sure, ver­ti­cal sta­bil­ity, and uni­form height. Canopy lay­er­ing and shape was of lit­tle im­por­tance for se­lec­tion. Orang­utans in­hab­it­ing highly frag­mented areas are im­por­tant for con­nec­tiv­ity of metapop­u­la­tions and can still per­sist in these areas, like palm-oil forests.

There are vast areas of un­in­hab­ited trop­i­cal for­est in both Bor­neo and Suma­tra as well as swamps, and re­searchers think this could come from a vast array of rea­sons. Thou­sands of years of hunt­ing in com­bi­na­tion with oc­ca­sional droughts caus­ing star­va­tion could have con­tributed to the va­cancy in these now high-qual­ity habi­tats. (An­cre­naz, et al., 2021; Davies, et al., 2017; Payne and Pru­dente, 2008; Sea­man, et al., 2019)

Sys­tem­atic and Tax­o­nomic His­tory

Orang­utans are clas­si­fied as mem­bers of the pla­cen­tal mam­mals, or in­fr­a­class Eu­the­ria. They are mem­bers of fam­ily ho­minidae, or the fam­ily con­tain­ing Great Apes and Hu­mans. Great Apes in­clude bono­bos, east­ern and west­ern go­ril­las, chim­panzees, and the orang­utans.

In 2009, re­searchers found that at least 13 mil­lion years ago, hu­mans and orang­utans shared a com­mon an­ces­tor al­though species from genus Pongo are the least re­lated great ape to hu­mans.

Based on his­tor­i­cal bio­geo­graph­i­cal re­con­struc­tion, the an­ces­tral pop­u­la­tion of orang­utan most likely came from main­land Asia and set­tled on the North­ern end of Suma­tra. Within the Suma­tran pop­u­la­tion, re­cently a new species has been ar­tic­u­lated sep­a­rate from P. abelii. Ge­netic ev­i­dence in com­bi­na­tion with de­vel­op­men­tal dif­fer­ences in skull size dis­tin­guish P. tapan­ulien­sis as a species and thus, one of the most en­dan­gered species of Great Ape, with per­haps only 800 in­di­vid­u­als in ex­is­tence today. How­ever, P. tapan­ulien­sis is also the old­est species of orang­utan, di­verg­ing about 3.38 mil­lion years ago. About 674 thou­sand years ago, ge­netic mod­el­ing in­di­cates that the Suma­tran and Bornean pop­u­la­tions split. (Gre­han and Schwartz, 2009; "Mor­pho­me­t­ric, Be­hav­ioral, and Ge­nomic Ev­i­dence for a New Orang­utan Species", 2017; Xin, et al., 2013; Xu and Ar­na­son, 1996)

  • Synonyms
    • Faunus (Oken, 1816)
    • Lophotus (Fischer, 1813)
    • Macrobates (Bilberg, 1828)
    • Satyrus (Lesson, 1840)
  • Synapomorphies
    • Exclusively Arboreal (unless male is too large for branches)
    • Cheek Flanges in males

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Orang­utans are char­ac­ter­ized by their ape-like ap­pear­ance and dis­tinc­tive red, shaggy hair that cov­ers al­most their en­tire body, with males and some fe­male pos­sess­ing a beard of the same color. Their long arms, up to 7 or 8 feet in length, also char­ac­ter­ize this genus. Males weigh around 160 pounds with fe­males around 80. They have ex­tremely flex­i­ble joints with full ro­ta­tion of their hips. Both their long arms and flex­i­ble hips aid in their quadru­manous scram­bling, which dif­fers from swing­ing from branches, and fea­tures in­di­vid­u­als shift­ing their weight to force branches to bend over to the next branch. Their weight is also note­wor­thy, with males weigh­ing up­wards of 300 pounds and fe­males at 150.

Most his­toric lit­er­a­ture is cur­rently aimed at dis­tin­guish­ing phys­i­cal char­ac­ter­is­tics of the Bornean and Suma­tran pop­u­la­tions of orang­utan. There are gen­eral trends be­tween the two is­lands, with Suma­tran in­di­vid­u­als hav­ing a less broad of a face and being slightly lighter than Bornean Orang­utans. Per­haps the biggest dif­fer­ence in the two re­gional pop­u­la­tions is the shape and tex­ture of the male cheek flanges (pads). Male orang­utans pos­sess cheek flaps later in de­vel­op­ment as a form of sex­ual se­lec­tion, with big­ger flanges in­di­cat­ing a more suc­cess­ful mate. Suma­tran males are noted to have a di­a­mond like shape of their flanges, with tufts of short yel­low or white hair. Bornean coun­ter­parts have a more square shape of the flanges, de­vel­op­ing more lat­er­ally and rounded. It has few hairs and wrin­kles the brow and creases the nose, giv­ing Bornean males a mostly grumpy ap­pear­ance. (Abrams, 2017; Mack­in­non, 1975)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • male larger
  • sexes shaped differently

Re­pro­duc­tion

Orang­utans are known for hav­ing the slow­est life his­tory of not only all great apes, but mam­mals in gen­eral. It takes an av­er­age of 15.4 years for a fe­male to be­come re­pro­duc­tively ac­tive, with a range of 13-18 years. Fe­males, on av­er­age, birth young about every 9 years.

Fe­males that are re­pro­duc­tively ac­tive are usu­ally trav­el­ing alone or with their off­spring. If there is a male and a fe­male trav­el­ing to­gether, it may be due to a sex­ual re­la­tion­ship or a co­in­ci­den­tal close­ness in prox­im­ity from feed­ing. Males, flanged or un­flanged, pur­sue fe­males that are re­pro­duc­tively re­cep­tive, and will fight for a re­pro­duc­tively ac­tive fe­male. Flanged males have cheek pads and longer vo­cal­iza­tions, as well as usu­ally being more dom­i­nant and sex­u­ally suc­cess­ful. How­ever, they are ex­tremely in­tol­er­ant of other males, using their vo­cal­iza­tions from throat pouches as a way to alert other males of their pres­ence.

The bi­ma­tur­ism of male orang­utans have im­pli­ca­tions on their court­ing be­hav­ior for a fe­male. As noted, flanged males use pri­mar­ily their vo­cal­iza­tions that deter males and at­tract a fe­male. Un­flanged males re­sort to search­ing for a fe­male.

Fe­male choice is the main fac­tor in mat­ing suc­cess. Older fe­males pre­fer more flanged, dom­i­nant males. Some­times, forced mat­ing oc­curs, and this is ac­com­pa­nied by re­sis­tance and/or a fight. Fe­males can be co­op­er­a­tive at the be­gin­ning and re­sis­tant to­wards the end or vice versa.

Once the fe­male is re­cep­tive to re­pro­duc­tion, an in­vol­un­tary con­sortship is formed for any­where from a few days to a month. Dur­ing this time, the male and fe­male travel to­gether, and both flanged and un­flanged males ex­hibit mate guard­ing be­hav­ior to keep other males away. How­ever, usu­ally more dom­i­nant, flanged males are more suc­cess­ful in mate guard­ing, while un­flanged males being kicked out by a larger male if en­coun­tered. (Payne and Pru­dente, 2008; Shu­maker, et al., 2008; S.S, et al., 2009)

Once im­preg­nated, ges­ta­tion for fe­male orang­utans lasts about 8 and a half months. There is no breed­ing sea­son for this genus, and fe­males are able to have off­spring year-round up until they die. As stated pre­vi­ously, fe­males have off­spring about every 9 years al­most al­ways to one, with very rarely hav­ing twins.

New­born orang­utans are ex­tremely de­pen­dent upon their mother, and weigh 3.5-4.5 pounds. Body con­tact is con­tin­u­ous for the first 3 months, but en­tire de­pen­dence on food lasts for the first two years. At around 3 years of age, young orang­utans make play nests and start ex­plor­ing their habi­tat while al­ways in view of the mother.

Full wean­ing of milk does not occur until about 3.5 years old. Young orang­utans will spend the next few years im­i­tat­ing their mother's be­hav­ior through trial and error. The mother will teach her young every­thing from swing­ing through trees, using tools for food, and over­all how to live in the for­est.

At about six or seven years of age, off­spring are con­sid­ered ado­les­cents and spend more and more time away from their mother. At this age, fe­males ex­press an in­ter­est in sex­ual se­lec­tion and can be re­pro­duc­tively ac­tive, al­though males tend to pur­sue older, more es­tab­lished fe­males. Fe­males may not give birth until they are 10-15 years of age. Nei­ther males or fe­males are fully grown at this age, and will oc­ca­sion­ally come into con­tact with their mother dur­ing this pe­riod. Their mother may have given birth to a sib­ling by then, giv­ing these ado­les­cents some­one to play with. Males will not re­pro­duce until they are 13-15 years old, de­spite being re­pro­duc­tively fer­tile well be­fore that. (Payne and Pru­dente, 2008; Shu­maker, et al., 2008; S.S, et al., 2009)

Orang­utans are semi-soli­tary in na­ture, so males have al­most no in­vest­ment or care into off­spring.

Fe­males de­velop a strong bond with their young, and tim­ing and in­vest­ment are stated in the pre­vi­ous sec­tion. When she gives birth to a new­born, she often shields it from view. A wild fe­male orang­utan may only have 3-5 off­spring in her en­tire life. (Payne and Pru­dente, 2008; Shu­maker, et al., 2008; S.S, et al., 2009)

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • female parental care
  • pre-fertilization
    • protecting
      • male
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • extended period of juvenile learning

Lifes­pan/Longevity

Orang­utans are fairly long-lived apes in the wild liv­ing up to 58 years for males and 53 for fe­males. In cap­tiv­ity, they are known to live as long, but the old­est cap­tive orang­utan is thought to have been 61 when she died.

Orang­utans most com­monly die of old age in the wild, but cur­rent hu­man-in­duced threats in­clude star­va­tion, hunt­ing by hu­mans as pests, cap­tur­ing ba­bies to be sold on the black mar­ket as pests, and wild­fires. (Tacutu, et al., 2017)

Be­hav­ior

As stated be­fore, mem­bers of the genus pongo are semi-soli­tary for most of their adult life. They are motile, and their day-to-day move­ments are largely a re­sponse to spa­tiotem­po­ral vari­abil­ity in food avail­abil­ity. They eat mainly ripe fruits and food dis­tri­b­u­tion is vari­able, so most orang­utans travel alone un­less it is a fe­male with her off­spring. Adult flanged males are the most in­tol­er­ant of other mem­bers of their species un­less they are ac­tively try­ing to form a con­sortship with an­other fe­male, thus they are rarely ter­ri­to­r­ial. Be­sides when mat­ing, orang­utans are tol­er­ant of other mem­bers of their species in their home range, how­ever males tend to stay away from each other more than fe­males. They are motile, and their day-to-day move­ments are largely a re­sponse to spa­tiotem­po­ral vari­abil­ity in food avail­abil­ity.

There is a so­cial hi­er­ar­chy, but flanged males are at the top. They can out­com­pete un­flanged males for a mate and will typ­i­cally choose an older fe­male rather than an ado­les­cent one.

Their lo­co­mo­tion through the trees is a dis­tin­guish­ing char­ac­ter­is­tic of these Great Apes. They climb, clamor, and brachi­ate through the canopy, in­gest­ing over 90% of their diet in this re­gion.

Tool use also de­fines orang­utans. Most stud­ies have fo­cused on the be­hav­ioral dif­fer­ences be­tween the two re­gional pop­u­la­tions of orang­utans, or Bornean and Suma­tran Is­land pop­u­la­tions. Most orang­utan off­spring learn all of their be­hav­iors from their mother, in­clud­ing tool use. In­ter­est­ingly, re­search has found that dif­fer­ent pop­u­la­tions of orang­utan dis­play dif­fer­ent be­hav­iors based off of their ge­o­graphic re­gion, an in­di­ca­tion of a cul­ture. Roof build­ing of nests (to stop rain) was ob­served in Bor­neo, but not on Suma­tra. There were other be­hav­iors, like cov­er­ing your­self in leaves to pro­tect from the sun, that were ob­served in some of the pop­u­la­tions on Bor­neo and Suma­tra, but not all.

Orang­utans on the is­land Suma­tra have gen­er­ally been ob­served using tools more than that of Bor­neo. These orang­utans are known to use sticks to poke out in­sects from trees and leaves to hold fruits with spines, as well as many oth­ers. Bornean orang­utans are gen­er­ally more so­cial, and thus in cap­tiv­ity, have learned a va­ri­ety of be­hav­iors from orang­utans and hu­mans that are ob­vi­ously not their species or di­rect mother. They have been known to make paint­ings in cap­tiv­ity as well as learn sign lan­guage, or as­so­ci­ate spe­cific signs with spe­cific human mean­ings. Re­ha­bil­i­tat­ing orang­utans in Tan­jung Na­tional Park in Kali­man­tan were seen im­i­tat­ing human be­hav­ior like clothes wash­ing, teeth brush­ing, ham­mer­ing nails, and vol­un­tar­ily rid­ing in boats. (Payne and Pru­dente, 2008; Schaik, et al., 2008)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion among mem­bers of genus Pongo is per­haps one of the most com­plex and di­verse char­ac­ter­is­tics of these an­i­mals. The most dis­tinct and charis­matic form of com­mu­ni­ca­tion for orang­utans is the "long call," per­formed by adult flanged males only. Heard from over a mile away, these long calls in­vite be­hav­ioral re­sponses from all ages and sexes, but other adult males re­spond the most. The flanged male uses their throat sac to emit a long, some­times min­utes long, se­ries of "roars." Their cheek pads fun­nel the sound in a par­tic­u­lar di­rec­tion, and stud­ies have shown males will some­times make these calls in the di­rec­tion they are trav­el­ing to warn other males they are on the move to­wards them as early as one day in ad­vance. Other males use their long calls in re­sponse to trees falling, mostly be­cause some males will knock down trees as a form of dom­i­nance. Some males use their long calls to at­tract fe­males, and it has been noted that Bornean fe­male orang­utans can rec­og­nize the calls of in­di­vid­ual males.

Be­sides long calls, there are many other forms of orang­utan vocal com­mu­ni­ca­tion. Bornean orang­utans were ob­served in 2019 to have 11 vocal sig­nal types. These vocal sig­nals can be any­thing from dis­plea­sure, to play­ful in­fants fight­ing, to warn­ing of rain­fall. In fact, Suma­tran orang­utans were ob­served sup­press­ing alarm calls up to 20 min­utes when wait­ing for threats to pass. This means they are ca­pa­ble time-space-dis­placed re­sponses or giv­ing in­for­ma­tion about an event that has hap­pened in the past.

Ges­tures are per­haps the most widely-used orang­utan form of com­mu­ni­ca­tions. Over 21 ges­ture types were iden­ti­fied in a Bornean pop­u­la­tion of orang­utans in 2019. They have a range of mean­ings, from re­quest­ing ob­jects, shar­ing tools, warn­ing of harm­ful be­hav­ior, climb over/on me, re­sume play, etc. Ges­tures can be made by hands or feet, though hands are pre­ferred. Orang­utans have also been known to re­peat ges­tures when their mes­sage was only par­tially un­der­stood, and in some cases can change their ges­tures if they be­lieve their mes­sage was not un­der­stood at all. (Cart­mill and Byrne, 2010; Galdikas and In­s­ley, 1988; Lameira and Call, 2018; Leav­ens, 2007; Payne and Pru­dente, 2008)

  • Other Communication Modes
  • mimicry

Food Habits

Orang­utans are highly op­por­tunis­tic for­agers, with over 317 food sources noted in 1988 with a com­plete species list num­ber­ing over 1600 dif­fer­ent species preyed upon by orang­utans. When re­sources are abun­dant, they pre­fer to con­sume fruits as 60% of the orang­utan diet is fruit, but this varies month to month. When fruit is avail­able, they con­sume a plethora of dif­fer­ent species like ficus and durian, the bulk of their diet, but they will eat ap­ples, or­anges, ba­nanas, berries, ly­chees, bread­fruit, and jack­fruit. With de­creas­ing habi­tat avail­able, many orang­utans are raid­ing farm­ers fruit crops, with fe­males being more likely to raid than males. In ad­di­tion, it's been found that the great­est fac­tor to their dis­tri­b­u­tion, which is linked to for­ag­ing, is rest­ing time. While much of their time is spent for­ag­ing, the pe­ri­ods of rest in be­tween are the main de­ter­mi­nant of dis­tri­b­u­tion.

When fruit isn't an op­tion, many orang­utans will in­crease from 22% of their for­ag­ing time for bark to 44%. In ad­di­tion to tree bark, species like Giron­niera ner­vosa are also an im­por­tant source of leaves and flow­ers. Most mem­bers of pongo pre­fer younger leaves and flow­ers, as many adult leaves de­velop tox­ins that can be harm­ful for her­bi­vores. They also con­sume nec­tar and honey from trees and plants.

Orang­utans don't only eat plants, con­sum­ing in­ver­te­brate species in­clude 4 species of ants, 4 species of ter­mites, 2 species of cater­pil­lars, leeches, mag­gots, ticks, and lar­vae. They poke out many of these in­ver­te­brates using a stick in the bur­rows or crevices where the in­ver­te­brates re­side. Orang­tu­ans will also eat ver­te­brates in­clud­ing the eggs of many bird species, tree rats, small lizards, and pri­mates like loras or gib­bons.

There are a few re­gional dif­fer­ences be­tween the orang­utan diet. Sub­pop­u­la­tions of orang­utans each vary widely on what they eat, as their for­ag­ing be­hav­ior de­ter­mines what they eat, and their be­hav­ior varies by pop­u­la­tion. For ex­am­ple, on the is­land of Suma­tra, figs are much more preva­lent, so Suma­tran orang­utans have a higher fig den­sity in their diet. (Camp­bell-Smith and Camp­bell-Smith, 2011; Carne, et al., 2015; Galdikas and In­s­ley, 1988; Rus­son, et al., 2009)

Pre­da­tion

There are few preda­tors for mem­bers of genus pongo be­sides hu­mans. Tigers and leop­ards are the main preda­tor of orang­utan, but it is very rare as most of these preda­tors have been ex­tir­pated from their orig­i­nal home ranges. Young orang­utans are at most risk of pre­da­tion, but being ar­bo­real, orang­utans are rarely pre­dated upon. Bornean orang­utans do not have to worry about tigers, as they have been erad­i­cated from the is­land for decades. If a preda­tor is spot­ted, many orang­utans will raise an alarm call for neigh­bor­ing orang­utans, often after the preda­tor has passed. (Lameira and Call, 2018; Schaik, et al., 2008)

  • Known Predators
    • Leopard
    • Tiger

Ecosys­tem Roles

With hun­dreds of plant and tree species pre­dated upon by the genus g. pongo, orang­utans are im­por­tant seed dis­persers for the forests they re­side in. These forests house many other spec­tac­u­lar and rare species, like the Suma­tran tiger or Suma­tran ele­phant. (Galdikas, 1988)

  • Ecosystem Impact
  • disperses seeds
  • creates habitat

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

All 3 species in this genus are con­sid­ered crit­i­cally en­dan­gered by the IUCN. Pro­jec­tions aren't up to date but es­ti­mate around 102,000 Bornean orang­utans, 13,800 Suma­tran orang­utans, and fewer than 800 Tapan­uli orang­utans. All major threats to mem­bers of pongo* are hu­man-in­duced or hu­man-re­lated, with the largest of these threats is habi­tat loss. Con­ver­sion of for­est to agri­cul­ture is the main form of habi­tat loss, with the Suma­tran orang­utan being heav­ily af­fected by palm-oil plan­ta­tions. Leg­is­la­tion like the Aceh Provin­cial Spa­tial Plan of 2013 dis­re­gards Suma­tran orang­utan habi­tat in favor of ex­pan­sion of palm oil plan­ta­tions into their ter­ri­tory. Hous­ing, tourism, and recre­ation de­vel­op­ment also threaten orang­utan habi­tat. Log­ging for tim­ber, cre­ation of roads, and min­ing op­er­a­tions all con­tribute to habi­tat frag­men­ta­tion. Orang­utans are also sub­ject to the il­le­gal pet trade, with in­ten­tional killing of adult fe­male orang­utans and the sell­ing of her or­phans on the black mar­ket. Fires on Bor­neo af­fect the Tapan­uli and Bornean orang­utans, like when 90% of Kutai Na­tional Park was burned and killed thou­sands of orang­utans in the process.

Con­ser­va­tion Ac­tion is cur­rently lack­ing for orang­utans. There is no sys­tem­atic mon­i­tor­ing scheme for any species of orang­utan. There are ac­tion re­cov­ery plans for all but the species P. pyg­maeus, or the Bornean Orang­utan. Con­ser­va­tion sites have been iden­ti­fied for all 3 species of orang­utan, but P. pyg­maeus is lack­ing sites over their en­tire range. While over 90% of the Suma­tran pop­u­la­tion is pro­tected, a mere 35% is pro­tected for Bornean species, and only 1-10% for the Tapan­uli orang­utan. How­ever, there is hope, as all species have been in­cluded in in­ter­na­tional leg­is­la­tion and are sub­ject to in­ter­na­tional trade con­trol. There have also been re­cent ed­u­ca­tion and aware­ness pro­grams for all 3 species. ("IUCN Red List", 16 Oc­to­ber 2017; "IUCN Red List", Feb­ru­ary 8 2016; "IUCN Red List", Oc­to­ber 11 2017)

  • IUCN Red List [Link]
    Not Evaluated

Con­trib­u­tors

Alexan­der Hey (au­thor), Col­orado State Uni­ver­sity, Tanya Dewey (ed­i­tor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor.

Glossary

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

arboreal

Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

carnivore

an animal that mainly eats meat

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
dominance hierarchies

ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

female parental care

parental care is carried out by females

folivore

an animal that mainly eats leaves.

frugivore

an animal that mainly eats fruit

granivore

an animal that mainly eats seeds

herbivore

An animal that eats mainly plants or parts of plants.

insectivore

An animal that eats mainly insects or spiders.

mimicry

imitates a communication signal or appearance of another kind of organism

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

nectarivore

an animal that mainly eats nectar from flowers

omnivore

an animal that mainly eats all kinds of things, including plants and animals

polyandrous

Referring to a mating system in which a female mates with several males during one breeding season (compare polygynous).

rainforest

rainforests, both temperate and tropical, are dominated by trees often forming a closed canopy with little light reaching the ground. Epiphytes and climbing plants are also abundant. Precipitation is typically not limiting, but may be somewhat seasonal.

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

solitary

lives alone

swamp

a wetland area that may be permanently or intermittently covered in water, often dominated by woody vegetation.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

year-round breeding

breeding takes place throughout the year

Ref­er­ences

08 Feb­ru­ary 2016. "Bornean Orang­utan" (On-line). IUCN Red List. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 15, 2021 at https://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​species/​17975/​123809220.

Feb­ru­ary 8 2016. "IUCN Red List" (On-line). Bornean Orang­utan. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 27, 2021 at https://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​species/​17975/​123809220.

16 Oc­to­ber 2017. "IUCN Red List" (On-line). Suma­tran Orang­utan. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 27, 2021 at https://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​species/​121097935/​123797627.

Oc­to­ber 11 2017. "IUCN Red List" (On-line). Tapan­uli Orang­utan. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 27, 2021 at https://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​species/​120588639/​120588662.

2017. Mor­pho­me­t­ric, Be­hav­ioral, and Ge­nomic Ev­i­dence for a New Orang­utan Species. Cur­rent Bi­ol­ogy, 27: 3487-3498.e10. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 15, 2021 at https://​www.​sciencedirect.​com/​science/​article/​pii/​S0960982217312459.

16 Oc­to­ber 2017. "Suma­tran Orang­utan" (On-line). IUCN Red List. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 15, 2021 at https://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​species/​121097935/​123797627.

11 Oc­to­ber 2017. "Tapan­uli Orang­utan" (On-line). IUCN Red List. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 15, 2021 at https://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​species/​120588639/​120588662.

Abrams, S. 2017. "New Eng­land Pri­mate Con­ser­vancy" (On-line). Suma­tran Orang­utan. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 29, 2021 at https://​www.​neprimateconservancy.​org/​sumatran-orangutan.​html.

An­cre­naz, M., F. Oram, N. Nardiy­ono, M. Slimi. 2021. Im­por­tance of Small For­est Frag­ments in Agri­cul­tural Land­scapes for Main­tain­ing Orang­utan Metapop­u­la­tions. Fron­tiers in Forests and Global Change, 4: 5. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 15, 2021 at https://​www.​frontiersin.​org/​articles/​10.​3389/​ffgc.​2021.​560944/​full.

Camp­bell-Smith, G., M. Camp­bell-Smith. 2011. Raiders of the Lost Bark: Orang­utan For­ag­ing Strate­gies in a De­graded Land­scape. PLoS ONE, Vol­ume 6, Issue 6: e20962. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 20, 2021 at https://​www.​ncbi.​nlm.​nih.​gov/​pmc/​articles/​PMC3120831/​.

Carne, C., S. Sem­ple, J. Lehmann. 2015. In­ves­ti­gat­ing Con­straints on the Sur­vival of Orang­utans Across Bor­neo and Suma­tra. Trop­i­cal Con­ser­va­tion Sci­ence, Vol­ume 8, Issue 4: 940-954. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 20, 2021 at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​194008291500800405.

Cart­mill, E., R. Byrne. 2010. Se­man­tics of pri­mate ges­tures: in­ten­tional mean­ings of orang­utan ges­tures. An­i­mal Cog­ni­tion, 13/6: 793-804. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 13, 2021 at 10.1007/s10071-010-0328-7.

Davies, A., M. An­cre­naz, F. Oram, G. Asner. 2017. Canopy struc­ture dri­ves orang­utan habi­tat se­lec­tion in dis­turbed Bornean forests. Pro­ceed­ings of the Na­tional Acad­emy of Sci­ences, 114: 8307-8312. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 15, 2021 at https://​www.​pnas.​org/​content/​114/​31/​8307.

Galdikas, B. 1988. Orang­utan diet, range, and ac­tiv­ity at Tan­jung Put­ing, Cen­tral Bor­neo. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, Vol­ume 9, Issue 1: 1-35. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 20, 2021 at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​BF02740195.

Galdikas, B., S. In­s­ley. 1988. The Fast Call of the Adult Male Orang­utan. Jour­nal of Mam­mal­ogy, 69/2: 371-375. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 13, 2021 at https://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​1381390.

Gre­han, J., J. Schwartz. 2009. Evo­lu­tion of the sec­ond orang­utan: phy­logeny and bio­geog­ra­phy of ho­minid ori­gins. Jour­nal of Bio­geog­ra­phy, 36: 1823-1844.

Lameira, A., J. Call. 2018. Time-space–dis­placed re­sponses in the orang­utan vocal sys­tem. Sci­ence Ad­vances, 4/11. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 13, 2021 at https://​www.​science.​org/​doi/​10.​1126/​sciadv.​aau3401.

Leav­ens, D. 2007. An­i­mal Cog­ni­tion: Mul­ti­modal Tac­tics of Orang­utan Com­mu­ni­ca­tion. Cur­rent Bi­ol­ogy, 17: R762-R764. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 13, 2021 at https://​www.​sciencedirect.​com/​science/​article/​pii/​S0960982207016880.

Mack­in­non, J. 1975. Dis­tin­guish­ing char­ac­ters of the in­su­lar forms of orang-utan. In­ter­na­tional Zoo Year­book, 15: 195-197. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 29, 2021 at https://​onlinelibrary.​wiley.​com/​doi/​abs/​10.​1111/​j.​1748-1090.​1975.​tb01398.​x.

Payne, J., C. Pru­dente. 2008. Orang­utans: Be­hav­ior, Ecol­ogy, and Con­ser­va­tion. North Amer­ica: The MIT Press.

Rus­son, A., S. Wich, M. An­cre­naz, T. Kanamori, C. Knott. 2009. Ge­o­graphic vari­a­tion in orang­utan diets. Orang­utans: ge­o­graphic vari­a­tion in be­hav­ioral ecol­ogy and con­ser­va­tion: 135-156. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 20, 2021 at https://​www.​zora.​uzh.​ch/​id/​eprint/​31255/​.

S.S, U., T. Setia, B. Goossens, J. S, C. Knott. 2009. Orang­utan mat­ing be­hav­ior and strate­gies. Orang­utans: Ge­o­graphic Vari­a­tion in Be­hav­ioral Ecol­ogy and Con­ser­va­tion, 1: 235-244. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 06, 2021 at https://​www.​researchgate.​net/​publication/​230823426_​Orangutan_​mating_​behavior_​and_​strategies.

Schaik, C., A. Mar­shall, S. Wich. 2008. Ge­o­graphic vari­a­tion in orang­utan be­hav­ior and bi­ol­ogy. Orang­utans: 351-361. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 13, 2021 at https://​oxford.​universitypressscholarship.​com/​view/​10.​1093/​acprof:​oso/​9780199213276.​001.​0001/​acprof-9780199213276-chapter-24.

Sea­man, D., H. Bernard, M. An­cre­naz, D. Coomes. 2019. Den­si­ties of Bornean orang‐utans (Pongo pyg­maeus morio) in heav­ily de­graded for­est and oil palm plan­ta­tions in Sabah, Bor­neo. Amer­i­can jour­nal of pri­ma­tol­ogy, 81: e23030–n/a.

Shu­maker, R., S. Wich, L. Perkins. 2008. Re­pro­duc­tive Life His­tory Traits of Fe­male Orang­utans (Pongo spp.). Pri­mate Re­pro­duc­tive Aging, 36: 147-161. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 06, 2021 at https://​www.​karger.​com/​Article/​FullText/​137705.

Tacutu, R., D. Thorn­ton, E. John­son, A. Bu­dovsky, D. Barado, T. Craig, E. Diana, G. Lehmann, D. Toren, J. Wang. 2017. "Human Age­ing and Ge­nomic Re­sources" (On-line). AnAge entry for Pongo pyg­maeus. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 30, 2021 at https://​genomics.​senescence.​info/​legal.​html#​citation.

Xin, M., J. Kel­ley, K. Eil­ert­son, S. Musharoff, J. De­gen­hardt. 2013. Pop­u­la­tion Ge­nomic Analy­sis Re­veals a Rich Spe­ci­a­tion and De­mo­graphic His­tory of Orang-utans (Pongo pyg­maeus and Pongo abelii). PLOS ONE, 10: e77175. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 22, 2021 at https://​journals.​plos.​org/​plosone/​article?​id=10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0077175.

Xu, X., U. Ar­na­son. 1996. The mi­to­chon­dr­ial DNA mol­e­cule of Suma­tran orang­utan and a mol­e­c­u­lar pro­posal for two (Bornean and Suma­tran) species of orang­utan. Jour­nal of Mol­e­c­u­lar Evo­lu­tion, 43: 431-437. Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 15, 2021 at https://​pubmed.​ncbi.​nlm.​nih.​gov/​8875856/​.