Progne subispurple martin

Ge­o­graphic Range

Progne subis, com­monly known as pur­ple mar­tins, in­hab­its the Nearc­tic re­gion and can be found across North and South Amer­ica. They are mi­gra­tory birds that breed in North and Cen­tral Amer­ica and over­win­ter in South Amer­ica. The north­ern ex­tent of the breed­ing range in­cludes the Cana­dian provinces of Al­berta, Saskatchewan, and south­ern Man­i­toba. Pur­ple mar­tins breed across the east­ern half of the United States, and also may be found along the Pa­cific coast­line in­clud­ing the en­tire Baja Penin­sula. (Braun­ing, 1992; Brown, 1997; John­ston, 1966)

Pur­ple mar­tins over­win­ter across most of South Amer­ica in­clud­ing the coun­tries of Colom­bia, Venezuela, Guyana, Suri­name, Ecuador, Peru, Brazil, Bo­livia, Paraguay and Ar­gentina. Their win­ter range has been recorded to be South Amer­i­can low­lands any­where east of the Andes Moun­tains. Con­cen­trated pop­u­la­tions have been found to win­ter in Bo­livia and some provinces of Brazil. There have been some records of pur­ple mar­tin pop­u­la­tions found in the British Isles, but these birds rarely mi­grate out­side of the Amer­i­cas. (Brown, 1997)

Habi­tat

Pur­ple mar­tins pre­fer open spaces that are sit­u­ated close to any water source, as they are in­sec­ti­vores and are at­tracted to the large pop­u­la­tions of in­sects near wet­lands, swamps, and wet mead­ows. Pur­ple mar­tins also seem to avoid high el­e­va­tions, for in­stance the Ap­palachian Moun­tains, but may be found at el­e­va­tions from less than 100 m to 4,000 m. Due to col­o­niza­tion and human in­ter­ac­tions in their nat­ural habi­tats, pur­ple mar­tins are now ac­cus­tomed to human in­ter­ac­tion and live in close prox­im­ity with hu­mans today. They tend to find shel­ter in urban set­tle­ments, often liv­ing in spe­cially made bird­houses called "mar­tin houses". His­tor­i­cally, this species in­hab­ited for­est edges, mon­tane forests, and deserts and nested in aban­doned wood­pecker cav­i­ties. Some pop­u­la­tions that breed in the west­ern United States con­tinue to live in these nat­ural set­tings, how­ever most uti­lize man-made mar­tin houses. (Braun­ing, 1992; Brown, 1997)

Dur­ing mi­gra­tion, these birds stopover in a va­ri­ety of habi­tats. They usu­ally fly over coastal lines and cross the Gulf of Mex­ico. They have been recorded in low­lands and the high moun­tain ranges of Venezuela and Co­lum­bia. They are often seen in cities and open areas while mi­grat­ing south. Win­ter­ing habi­tats in­clude rain­forests, agri­cul­tural areas, and clear­ings of South Amer­ica. They may also re­side in urban plazas. (Braun­ing, 1992; Brown, 1997)

  • Aquatic Biomes
  • lakes and ponds
  • rivers and streams
  • temporary pools
  • Range elevation
    <100 to 4000 m
    to 13123.36 ft
  • Average elevation
    2600 m
    8530.18 ft

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Pur­ple mar­tins are the largest North Amer­i­can swal­low, mea­sur­ing 20.3 cm long and weigh­ing 56 g on av­er­age. Their wingspan is about 45.7 cm. Males are a bit larger, en­tirely shiny, and deep pur­ple or al­most black in col­oration. Pur­ple mar­tin males have less shine on their black­ish wings and tails com­pared to their heads and backs. Fe­males are over­all gray or gray-blue with darker wings and crown feath­ers, and fea­ture a white breast smudged with vary­ing de­grees of gray. West­ern fe­males are over­all paler than east­ern. Pur­ple mar­tins have a dark, black-brown bill and the av­er­age length is 8.2 mm for males and 8.5 mm for fe­males. Their gape tends to be yel­low in young birds and a dull or­angey-brown color in adults. Their legs and feet have a black brown col­oration and their eyes are dark brown. (Brown, 1997; Lay­ton, 1972)

Ju­ve­nile pur­ple mar­tins are over­all gray to black with a white belly and gray-streaked breast. In ju­ve­niles, there is a much clearer line be­tween their gray throats and white bel­lies com­pared to fe­males which will have a pri­mar­ily gray belly. In flight, a ju­ve­nile's tail may have a nar­row, slight fork whereas adult tails are dis­tinctly forked and wider. (Brown, 1997; Lay­ton, 1972)

Pop­u­la­tions re­sid­ing in the south­west United States ex­hibit lighter col­oration than pur­ple mar­tins in other re­gions. This lighter col­oration is hy­poth­e­sized to be an adap­ta­tion to the desert cli­mate and serves to ab­sorb less heat. (Lay­ton, 1972; Wade, 1966)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • male larger
  • sexes colored or patterned differently
  • male more colorful
  • Range mass
    10 to 55 g
    0.35 to 1.94 oz
  • Average mass
    48 g
    1.69 oz
  • Range length
    18 to 22 cm
    7.09 to 8.66 in
  • Average length
    20.3 cm
    7.99 in
  • Range wingspan
    39 to 43 cm
    15.35 to 16.93 in
  • Range basal metabolic rate
    2.6 KJ*g^-1*day^-1 to 4.0 KJ*g^-1*day^-1 cm3.O2/g/hr
  • Average basal metabolic rate
    3.0 KJ*g^-1*day^-1 cm3.O2/g/hr

Re­pro­duc­tion

Pur­ple mar­tins are so­cially monog­a­mous, but form a pair bond that rarely ex­tends to sub­se­quent sea­sons. In Texas, 5% of pur­ple mar­tins that oc­cu­pied more than one nest prac­ticed polygamy. Males ar­rive at the breed­ing grounds first and will se­lect nest cav­i­ties where he will dis­play to po­ten­tial mates. The male often se­lects two nest sites for fe­males to choose from. Males are very ter­ri­to­r­ial and will ag­gres­sively de­fend nest sites from other males. Un­paired males will per­form an aer­ial dis­play to any nearby fe­male. This dis­play be­gins with the male fly­ing out from his nest in a wide arc, then swoop­ing back into the cav­ity, pop­ping out his head and singing. Re­search has sug­gested that de­spite these ef­forts, fe­males are more in­ter­ested in the qual­ity of a nest site than a male's dis­play­ing abil­ity. Once a pair has formed, the male de­fends the nest cav­ity as well as his mate. Al­though the pair is ag­gres­sive to­wards for­eign in­trud­ers, they tol­er­ate each other and will con­tinue to tol­er­ate each other in sub­se­quent years even when they are paired with other mates. (Brown, 1979; Brown, 1997; John­ston and Hardy, 1962)

Gen­er­ally pur­ple mar­tins spend their win­ters in South Amer­ica just past the Andes, and start re­turn­ing to their north­ern breed­ing grounds as early as Jan­u­ary 1. They slowly move north­wards pro­gress­ing gen­er­ally 3 to 5 de­grees lat­i­tude (330 to to 550 km) every half a month and they reach their north­ern lim­its around May 1. Adult males often re­turn first, fol­lowed shortly by adult fe­males while sub-adults re­turn a cou­ple weeks after. This gen­eral breed­ing pat­tern has been found to be true for a gen­er­al­ized pop­u­la­tion, ex­cept the non-colo­nial Saguaro desert broods. These pur­ple mar­tins ar­rive early May, which is about two and a half months later than any other colony at the same lat­i­tude. Rea­sons for the dif­fer­ence in ar­rival pat­terns have not yet been dis­cov­ered. (John­ston and Hardy, 1962; Mor­ton and Der­rick­son, 1990)

After a pair bond is formed, the mar­tins can start build­ing a nest. Nest build­ing starts about a month be­fore the pair in­tends to lay the eggs. Com­mon ma­te­ri­als used to build nests are green leaves, grass, sticks, paper, mud, and feath­ers. The use of green leaves as nest­ing ma­te­ri­als is poorly un­der­stood, but there are cur­rently many hy­pothe­ses. The fe­male per­forms most of the con­struc­tion, while the male gath­ers ma­te­ri­als and de­fends the cav­ity from other mar­tins. His­tor­i­cally, pur­ple mar­tins used nat­ural cav­i­ties to nest in but due to de­for­esta­tion and the re­moval of dead trees, these birds mostly nest within man-made "mar­tin houses" that can house an en­tire colony of mar­tins. (Alsop III, 2002)

The breed­ing sea­son for pur­ple mar­tins starts in May and will last until June. A pur­ple mar­tin nest can have any­where from 3 to 8 white, oval eggs but the av­er­age amount of eggs laid is 5. These eggs are usu­ally about 2.4 by 1.7 cm in size and are then in­cu­bated for 15 to 18 days. The chicks fledge after 26 to 31 days and travel in a fam­ily group. The group re­turns to sleep at the nest for sev­eral days. At 7 to 10 days after fledg­ing the young are able to sur­vive on their own and will dis­perse. Young pur­ple mar­tins can re­pro­duce in the first sub­se­quent breed­ing sea­son. (Alsop III, 2002; Braun­ing, 1992; Brown, 1979; Brown, 1997; Lay­ton, 1972)

  • Breeding interval
    Purple martins generally breed once yearly.
  • Breeding season
    Purple martin breeding season occurs during the months of May and June.
  • Range eggs per season
    3 to 8
  • Average eggs per season
    4 to 5
  • Average eggs per season
    4
    AnAge
  • Range time to hatching
    15 to 18 days
  • Range fledging age
    26 to 31 days
  • Range time to independence
    33 to 41 days
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    <1 years
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    <1 years

After the eggs are laid, the fe­male is the pri­mary in­cu­ba­tor. In­cu­ba­tion lasts from 15 to 18 days. When the fe­male leaves the nest, the male will pro­ceed in in­cu­bat­ing the eggs him­self but this hap­pens in­fre­quently. As soon as the chicks hatch, brood­ing be­gins and usu­ally lasts until the tenth day. The fe­male alone broods the young. Pur­ple mar­tin chicks are al­tri­cial and are com­pletely de­pen­dent upon the par­ents for sur­vival. (Alsop III, 2002; Brown, 1980; Brown, 1997; Lay­ton, 1972)

The young are fed within hours of hatch­ing and will con­tinue to be fed for 5 to 7 days after the young fledge. Both the male and fe­male feed the brood. Feed­ing oc­curs by re­gur­gi­tat­ing food and trans­fer­ring it into the mouths of the young one by one. As the brood gets older the feed­ing ses­sions be­come more fre­quent and reaches its peak when the young start gain­ing the most weight, which oc­curs around days 17 to 21. The feed­ing be­comes so reg­u­lar that it may occur every 30 sec­onds. Par­ents en­sure that the food is the proper size to be swal­lowed and if the pieces are too large and swal­low­ing does not occur in­stantly, the food is re­moved from their mouths. Par­ents keep the nest clean by eat­ing the fecal sacs and en­cour­ag­ing the young to defe­cate by pok­ing at their cloa­cal re­gion. After two weeks of de­vel­op­ment, the fe­male pur­ple mar­tin will cease to sleep in the same com­part­ment as the nestlings, be­cause less fre­quent night brood­ing is nec­es­sary. (Alsop III, 2002; Brown, 1980; Brown, 1997; Lay­ton, 1972)

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • male parental care
  • female parental care
  • pre-fertilization
    • provisioning
    • protecting
      • male
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female

Lifes­pan/Longevity

The longevity of pur­ple mar­tins range from 0 to 13 years and nine months. Pur­ple mar­tin mor­tal­ity is often the re­sult of se­vere weather. Three to four days of se­vere weather can lead to in­sect num­bers dras­ti­cally de­clin­ing. If there is a lack of food, pur­ple mar­tins can­not sur­vive and this often re­sults in pop­u­la­tion de­cline. An­other hin­drance to long life ex­pectancy is often body par­a­sites. Pur­ple mar­tins host a pro­to­zoan blood par­a­site Haemo­pro­teus prognei. This par­a­site can have dis­as­trous ef­fects on the sur­viv­ing rate of first year birds dur­ing the win­ter and mi­gra­tion pe­riod. (Brown, 1997; Lay­ton, 1972)

Be­hav­ior

Pur­ple mar­tins are mi­gra­tory birds that travel from cen­tral North Amer­ica to north­ern South Amer­ica to breed and over­win­ter, re­spec­tively. They are ac­tive dur­ing the day and are most ac­tive at dawn and dusk. They are colo­nial birds that live to­gether in large colonies. They ei­ther choose to fly around in pairs or stay in their colo­nial groups. At the end of the sum­mer the whole colony com­mutes back to South Amer­ica. These com­mu­nal flights home can in­clude tens of thou­sands of birds. (Alsop III, 2002; John­ston and Hardy, 1962)

Pur­ple mar­tins have shown dis­tinct group be­hav­ior. Mar­tin pairs are monog­a­mous and very ter­ri­to­r­ial, but when they join a colony they can show strong group-ori­en­ta­tion and pro­tec­tive­ness of the colony's ter­ri­tory. The colony so­cial­izes to­gether by ex­plor­ing the en­vi­ron­ment around them. This in­cludes vis­it­ing other colonies, scop­ing out po­ten­tial food sources, and vis­it­ing nearby streams and swamps. The so­cial­iz­ing groups con­sist of 3 or more pur­ple mar­tins. Pur­ple mar­tins for­age to­gether by hunt­ing in large flocks. When the flocks do find food they call the rest of the colony to come feed by mak­ing dis­tinct sounds. (Alsop III, 2002; John­ston and Hardy, 1962)

Pur­ple mar­tins also dis­play syn­chrony when they live in large colonies. Colo­nial pur­ple mar­tins work to­gether in an ef­fort to in­crease the re­pro­duc­tive suc­cess of the colony. Larger colonies have more suc­cess at achiev­ing syn­chrony, be­cause they are usu­ally made up of ma­ture birds, where as smaller colonies are made up of younger adults with less ex­pe­ri­ence. Syn­chrony is re­lated to age and is re­flected bet­ter in birds of sim­i­lar age. A colony of pur­ple mar­tins will achieve syn­chrony bet­ter if the birds are sim­i­lar in age. Colo­nial liv­ing does carry the risks of in­creased dis­ease trans­mis­sion and re­source com­pe­ti­tion. (Alsop III, 2002; John­ston and Hardy, 1962)

Pur­ple mar­tins are known to act ag­gres­sively when they are threat­ened. For in­stance, they will de­fend them­selves by peck­ing and flap­ping their wings try­ing to en­cour­age the un­wanted vis­i­tor to leave. When pur­ple mar­tins are threat­ened they take on a “Hor­i­zon­tal Threat Pos­ture”, ori­en­tat­ing their bod­ies so that it runs par­al­lel with the sur­face and press­ing their feath­ers down close to their bod­ies. Often when threat­ened, pur­ple mar­tins alert other mar­tins by singing. An­other de­fense mech­a­nism pur­ple mar­tins re­sort to is gap­ing. Gap­ing is when a pur­ple mar­tin opens its mouth wide and di­rects it to the po­ten­tial threat. Pur­ple mar­tins re­sort to lung­ing as an in­tim­i­da­tion ef­fect and will also try to in­tim­i­date a threat by bill snap­ping. (Alsop III, 2002; John­ston and Hardy, 1962)

  • Range territory size
    2.8 to 664 km^2

Home Range

Pur­ple mar­tins often re­turn to the same nest­ing site where they had lived the pre­vi­ous year. There is no in­for­ma­tion on how far these birds will travel while for­ag­ing. Pur­ple mar­tins have the abil­ity to fly far dis­tances to get to their nests. This sug­gests that these birds must have a large home range. Pur­ple mar­tins are known to travel from 2.8 to 664 km to get back to their nests. (Brown, 1997)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

Pur­ple mar­tins com­mu­ni­cate vo­cally and vi­su­ally. Pur­ple mar­tins have eleven iden­ti­fied vo­cal­iza­tions that they use for dif­fer­ent oc­ca­sions like mat­ing, warn­ing, and teach­ing the young dur­ing fledg­ing. Male pur­ple mar­tins use singing and vi­sual dis­plays to at­tract po­ten­tial fe­male mates. Ju­ve­nile pur­ple mar­tins re­sort to ‘choo-choo’ calls to at­tract their par­ents’ at­ten­tion if pro­tec­tion is needed. Par­ents use ju­ve­nile 'choo-choo' calls to as­sem­ble their broods and re­turn them safely to the nest. Fe­male pur­ple mar­tins use the ‘choo’ call to lead their young to and from group­ings areas dur­ing the fledg­ing pe­riod. Pur­ple mar­tins only re­sort to ‘zwarck’ calls when they need to send a high in­ten­sity alarm, and it is often ac­com­pa­nied by the birds div­ing straight down to­wards the in­vader. Male pur­ple mar­tins use ‘hee-hee’ vo­cal­iza­tions to fight off in­trud­ers. ‘Zweet’ calls are used to show in­traspe­cific ex­cite­ment, as well as send an alarm to warn other pur­ple mar­tins of a po­ten­tial threat and to en­cour­age them to fly away. Pur­ple mar­tins use ‘cher’ calls to com­mu­ni­cate daily and will use ‘chor­tle’ calls in high ex­cite­ment sit­u­a­tions. Males at­tract fe­males by singing ‘croak songs’ as well as to warn off un­mated males from en­ter­ing their ter­ri­tory. Dur­ing courtship males make a click­ing sound by snap­ping their lower and upper mandibles to­gether. The last vo­cal­iza­tion male pur­ple mar­tins use are ‘sub­songs’ and are heard dur­ing feed­ing and pre-mi­gra­tory pe­ri­ods. ‘Sub­songs’ are used to com­mu­ni­cate with other pur­ple mar­tins while so­cial­iz­ing to­gether. Pur­ple mar­tin males that are part of a sta­ble colony often per­form a 'dawn­song' which in­clude a va­ri­ety of sounds early in the morn­ing. (Brown, 1978; Brown, 1984; Brown, 1997; John­ston and Hardy, 1962; Mor­ton, 1985)

Food Habits

Pur­ple mar­tins are pri­mar­ily in­sec­ti­vores. They cap­ture food in flight and rarely glean in­sects from fo­liage or the ground. Pur­ple mar­tins pre­fer eat­ing fruit flies (Cer­ati­tis), mos­qui­toes (Culi­ci­dae), wasps (Polistes), bee­tles (Coleoptera), ants (Formi­ci­dae), grasshop­pers (Or­thoptera), ci­cadas (Ci­ca­di­dae) as well as drag­on­flies (Anisoptera). Pur­ple mar­tins may con­sume 400 flies or 2000 mos­qui­toes in a day. (Brown, 1997; Camp­bell Fin­lay, 1976; Lay­ton, 1972)

Pur­ple mar­tins rarely eat spi­ders Araneida and pre­fer any other in­sect in­stead. Gen­er­ally pur­ple mar­tin diets con­sists of 23% wasps and bees (Hy­menoptera), 16% flies, 15% as­sorted bugs like stink bugs (Pen­tato­mi­dae) and black bugs (Thyre­o­cori­dae), and 12% were bee­tles (On­thoph­a­gus). Pur­ple mar­tins also eat but­ter­flies and moths (Lep­i­doptera) but drag­on­flies seem to be pre­ferred. Young pur­ple mar­tins pre­fer eat­ing drag­on­flies over other in­sects while adults show no spe­cific pref­er­ence to drag­on­flies. (Brown, 1997; Camp­bell Fin­lay, 1976; Lay­ton, 1972)

Pur­ple mar­tins are greatly de­pen­dent on the weather, since it has a pro­found ef­fect on in­sect pop­u­la­tions. At low tem­per­a­tures in­sect food sources tend to de­crease, where as at high tem­per­a­ture pur­ple mar­tins have an abun­dance of food. High ve­loc­ity winds also de­crease food avail­abil­ity. Pur­ple mar­tins tend to eat bee­tles through­out all the sea­sons, but flies tend to dis­ap­pear from their diet in late Au­gust. In­sect pop­u­la­tions tend to be at its high­est dur­ing Au­gust which co­in­cides with greater nu­tri­tional need in prepa­ra­tion for fall mi­gra­tion. It is dur­ing Au­gust that pur­ple mar­tins must hoard up food and nu­tri­ents for the long flight back south. These birds con­sume water in flight by skim­ming their lower beaks across any water source. (Brown, 1997; Camp­bell Fin­lay, 1976; Lay­ton, 1972)

  • Animal Foods
  • insects
  • terrestrial non-insect arthropods

Pre­da­tion

The most com­mon preda­tors for pur­ple mar­tins are owls and snakes which prey on both adults and ju­ve­niles. Owls at­tack while the birds are in­side the nest. Owls grab the nest and shake it, which dis­ori­en­tates the pur­ple mar­tin. Pur­ple mar­tins then try to slip out of the en­trance hole, but owls grabs them with their claw. Hu­mans can help pre­vent these owl at­tacks by at­tach­ing curved rods over nest en­trances so that owls can­not perch atop the mar­tin houses. Owls have also been known to reach their claws into mar­tin houses to grab pur­ple mar­tins. (Brown, 1997; Kostka, 2000; Lay­ton, 1972)

Preda­tors like snakes or rac­coons are able to climb the bird house poles and make their way to the en­trance cav­i­ties. They pull out any adult birds and then pro­ceed to eat the eggs. The snakes that tend to prey on pur­ple mar­tins are usu­ally non-poi­so­nous and often climb up the poles and eat both the eggs and young. Rat snakes are the most com­mon snake preda­tors. Hawks and blue herons are the only preda­tors that prey on pur­ple mar­tins in the air. Do­mes­tic cats prey on pur­ple mar­tins when they are on the ground in search of nest­ing ma­te­r­ial. Squir­rels also prey on pur­ple mar­tins by climb­ing up the nest and en­ter­ing the cav­ity. Squir­rels kill the young, break up all the eggs and can even oc­cupy the nest to raise their own young. (Brown, 1997; Kostka, 2000; Lay­ton, 1972)

One anti-pre­da­tion be­hav­ior shown by pur­ple mar­tins is vig­i­lant nest clean­ing. Pur­ple mar­tin par­ents will eat fecal sacs and en­cour­age ju­ve­nile birds to defe­cate by pok­ing at their cloa­cal re­gion. The feces will be ei­ther con­sumed or re­moved from the nest by drop­ping them out­side. The elim­i­na­tion of feces and fecal sacs al­lows for pro­tec­tion, since the scent trails would be re­moved. (Brown, 1997; Kostka, 2000; Lay­ton, 1972)

Pur­ple mar­tins re­spond to preda­tor at­tacks by send­ing the 'zweet' call. 'Zweet' calls are used to warn other pur­ple mar­tins of the threat or to en­cour­age them to fly away. Pur­ple mar­tins often dive bomb their at­tacker. Pur­ple mar­tin colonies have no co­or­di­nated re­sponse to preda­tors. They do not all at­tack the preda­tor but do as­sem­ble as a crowd to con­fuse preda­tors and make it dif­fi­cult to focus on one bird. The only birds that do at­tack the preda­tor are the own­ers of a threat­ened nest. Pur­ple mar­tins will gen­er­ally stay out of the preda­tor’s way un­less the preda­tor comes within a few me­ters of their nest and young. Then pur­ple mar­tin adults have been known to at­tack the preda­tor. Pur­ple mar­tins ben­e­fit from liv­ing in large colonies, be­cause it adds to their pro­tec­tion and sta­bil­ity. Large colonies are able to de­tect preda­tors faster, thereby de­creas­ing pre­da­tion. (Brown, 1997; Kostka, 2000; Lay­ton, 1972)

Ecosys­tem Roles

Pur­ple mar­tins play roles as preda­tor, prey, com­peti­tor, and host. Pur­ple mar­tins are in­sec­tiv­o­rous and are known to be ef­fec­tive pest con­trollers. Pur­ple mar­tins are preyed upon by many species and also serve as a host to sev­eral species of par­a­sites. Pur­ple mar­tin pop­u­la­tions have been greatly af­fected by mite par­a­sites, specif­i­cally Der­manys­sus prognephilus that live in­side their nests. These blood-feed­ing par­a­sites are able to de­crease clutch size and an out­break may lead to colony aban­don­ment. Other par­a­sites in­clude ticks, bee­tles, louse­flies, fleas, and bowflies. (Brown, 1997; Kostka, 2000; Lay­ton, 1972; Moss and Camin, 1970; Wade, 1966)

Pur­ple mar­tins have to com­pete for nest­ing sites with house spar­rows and Eu­ro­pean star­lings. Star­lings often cor­ner pur­ple mar­tins in their own nest cav­i­ties where fight­ing re­sults and often ends in death. This com­pe­ti­tion is par­tic­u­larly un­for­tu­nate, as both house spar­rows and Eu­ro­pean star­lings are in­va­sive species in the United States and often out-com­pete na­tive pur­ple mar­tins for nest­ing habi­tat. (Brown, 1997; Kostka, 2000; Lay­ton, 1972; Moss and Camin, 1970; Wade, 1966)

Com­men­sal/Par­a­sitic Species

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

Hu­mans ben­e­fit largely from the in­sec­tiv­o­rous food habits of pur­ple mar­tins. Pur­ple mar­tins con­sume large quan­ti­ties of pest species in­clud­ing flies, stink bugs, clover wee­vil bee­tles, and mos­qui­toes. (Wade, 1966)

  • Positive Impacts
  • controls pest population

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

There are no known ad­verse ef­fects of pur­ple mar­tins on hu­mans.

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

The IUCN Red List has listed pur­ple mar­tins as a species of least con­cern. The United States Fed­eral list has listed pur­ples mar­tins as being a species of con­cern. Cur­rently sev­eral groups are work­ing on con­serv­ing the nat­ural habi­tat of pur­ple mar­tins. Under the Michi­gan Spe­cial An­i­mal list, pur­ple mar­tins are not in dan­ger. Over­all, pur­ple mar­tins have sta­ble pop­u­la­tion num­bers and in­habit a wide ge­o­graph­i­cal range. In Canada, pur­ple mar­tin pop­u­la­tions have seen some de­cline, and are cur­rently con­sid­ered at risk in British Co­lum­bia. Local pop­u­la­tions have suf­fered greatly from weather re­lated mor­tal­i­ties in the north­ern edges of the breed­ing range. Pur­ple mar­tins are also de­clin­ing due to the com­pe­ti­tion for nest­ing sites. Pur­ple mar­tins com­pete with in­va­sive house spar­rows and Eu­ro­pean star­lings for nest­ing sites. Cur­rently in British Co­lum­bia they are set­ting up spe­cial nest­ing boxes in the hope of sus­tain­ing the re­main­ing pop­u­la­tion. (Brown, 1997; Fraser, et al., 1997)

Con­trib­u­tors

Na­dine Sny­man (au­thor), Uni­ver­sity of Al­berta, Au­gus­tana Cam­pus, Doris Audet (ed­i­tor), Uni­ver­sity of Al­berta, Au­gus­tana Cam­pus, Rachelle Ster­ling (ed­i­tor), Spe­cial Pro­jects, Cather­ine Kent (ed­i­tor), Spe­cial Pro­jects.

Glossary

Nearctic

living in the Nearctic biogeographic province, the northern part of the New World. This includes Greenland, the Canadian Arctic islands, and all of the North American as far south as the highlands of central Mexico.

World Map

Neotropical

living in the southern part of the New World. In other words, Central and South America.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

agricultural

living in landscapes dominated by human agriculture.

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

arboreal

Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

bog

a wetland area rich in accumulated plant material and with acidic soils surrounding a body of open water. Bogs have a flora dominated by sedges, heaths, and sphagnum.

carnivore

an animal that mainly eats meat

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

choruses

to jointly display, usually with sounds, at the same time as two or more other individuals of the same or different species

colonial

used loosely to describe any group of organisms living together or in close proximity to each other - for example nesting shorebirds that live in large colonies. More specifically refers to a group of organisms in which members act as specialized subunits (a continuous, modular society) - as in clonal organisms.

crepuscular

active at dawn and dusk

desert or dunes

in deserts low (less than 30 cm per year) and unpredictable rainfall results in landscapes dominated by plants and animals adapted to aridity. Vegetation is typically sparse, though spectacular blooms may occur following rain. Deserts can be cold or warm and daily temperates typically fluctuate. In dune areas vegetation is also sparse and conditions are dry. This is because sand does not hold water well so little is available to plants. In dunes near seas and oceans this is compounded by the influence of salt in the air and soil. Salt limits the ability of plants to take up water through their roots.

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
duets

to jointly display, usually with sounds in a highly coordinated fashion, at the same time as one other individual of the same species, often a mate

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

female parental care

parental care is carried out by females

forest

forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.

insectivore

An animal that eats mainly insects or spiders.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

male parental care

parental care is carried out by males

marsh

marshes are wetland areas often dominated by grasses and reeds.

migratory

makes seasonal movements between breeding and wintering grounds

monogamous

Having one mate at a time.

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

mountains

This terrestrial biome includes summits of high mountains, either without vegetation or covered by low, tundra-like vegetation.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

oviparous

reproduction in which eggs are released by the female; development of offspring occurs outside the mother's body.

riparian

Referring to something living or located adjacent to a waterbody (usually, but not always, a river or stream).

seasonal breeding

breeding is confined to a particular season

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

suburban

living in residential areas on the outskirts of large cities or towns.

swamp

a wetland area that may be permanently or intermittently covered in water, often dominated by woody vegetation.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

temperate

that region of the Earth between 23.5 degrees North and 60 degrees North (between the Tropic of Cancer and the Arctic Circle) and between 23.5 degrees South and 60 degrees South (between the Tropic of Capricorn and the Antarctic Circle).

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

territorial

defends an area within the home range, occupied by a single animals or group of animals of the same species and held through overt defense, display, or advertisement

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

tropical savanna and grassland

A terrestrial biome. Savannas are grasslands with scattered individual trees that do not form a closed canopy. Extensive savannas are found in parts of subtropical and tropical Africa and South America, and in Australia.

savanna

A grassland with scattered trees or scattered clumps of trees, a type of community intermediate between grassland and forest. See also Tropical savanna and grassland biome.

temperate grassland

A terrestrial biome found in temperate latitudes (>23.5° N or S latitude). Vegetation is made up mostly of grasses, the height and species diversity of which depend largely on the amount of moisture available. Fire and grazing are important in the long-term maintenance of grasslands.

urban

living in cities and large towns, landscapes dominated by human structures and activity.

visual

uses sight to communicate

Ref­er­ences

U.S De­part­ment of Agri­cul­ture. Pur­ple Mar­tin Bird­house. 204,523. New York, NY: Ar­mond In­dus­tries, Inc.. 1971.

Alsop III, F. 2002. Pur­ple Mar­tin. Pp. "464" in R Green­berg, J Hamil­ton, eds. Birds of Canada, Vol. I, 1st Edi­tion. Toronto Ont.: South­ern lights Cus­tom Pub­lish­ing.

Braun­ing, D. 1992. Atlas of Breed­ing Birds in Penn­syl­va­nia. Pitts­burgh: Uni­ver­sity of Pitts­burgh Press.

Brown, C. 1978. Post-Fledg­ing Be­hav­ior of Pur­ple Mar­tins. The Wil­son Bul­letin, 90/3: "376-385". Ac­cessed No­vem­ber 01, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​4161087.

Brown, C. 1980. Sleep­ing Be­hav­ior of Pur­ple Mar­tins. The Con­dor, 82/2: "170-175". Ac­cessed No­vem­ber 01, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​1367472.

Brown, C. 1979. Ter­ri­to­ri­al­ity in the Pur­ple Mar­tin. The Wil­son Bul­letin, 91/4: "583-593". Ac­cessed No­vem­ber 01, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​4161272.

Brown, C. 1997. Birds Of North Amer­ica. Philadel­phia, PA: The Amer­i­can Or­nithol­o­gists/ Union and The Acad­emy of Nat­ural Sci­ences.

Brown, C. 1984. Vo­cal­iza­tions of the Pur­ple Mar­tin. The Con­dor, 86/4: 433-442. Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 03, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​1366823.

Camp­bell Fin­lay, J. 1976. Some Ef­fects of Weather on Pur­ple Mar­tin Ac­tiv­ity. The Auk, 93/2: "231-244". Ac­cessed Oc­to­ber 31, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​4085041.

Davis, J., C. Brown. 1999. Costs Of Colo­nial­ity And The Ef­fect Of Colony Size On Re­pro­duc­tive Suc­cess In Pur­ple Mar­tins. The Con­dor, 101/4: "737-745". Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 16, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​1370060.

Fraser, D., C. Sid­dle, D. Cop­ley, E. Wal­ters. 1997. The Re­turn of the Pur­ple Mar­tin in British Co­lum­bia. Wildlife Branch, British Co­lum­bia Min­istry of En­vi­ron­ment, Lands and Parks, 1: "279-282". Ac­cessed No­vem­ber 07, 2010 at http://​www.​env.​gov.​bc.​ca/​wld/​documents/​ce07fraser2.​pdf.

John­ston, R. 1966. The Adap­tive Basis of Ge­o­graphic Vari­a­tion in Color of The Pur­ple Mar­tin. The Con­dor, 68/3: "219-228". Ac­cessed No­vem­ber 07, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​1365555.

John­ston, R., J. Hardy. 1962. Be­hav­ior of the Pur­ple Mar­tin. The Wil­son Bul­letin, Vol 64, No. 3: "243-262". Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 25, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​4159068.

Kostka, K. 2000. Owl Guards for Gourds: A Way to Pro­tect Mar­tin Gourds from Aer­ial Preda­tors. Pur­ple Mar­tin Up­date, 9/4: "1".

Lay­ton, R. 1972. The Pur­ple Mar­tin. Jack­son, Mis­sisippi: Na­ture Books Pub­lish­ers.

Mor­ton, E. 1985. Vocal Im­i­ta­tion in a Cap­tive Pur­ple Mar­tin. The Wil­son Bul­letin, 97/3: "392-395". Ac­cessed No­vem­ber 01, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​4162123.

Mor­ton, E., K. Der­rick­son. 1990. The Bi­o­log­i­cal Sig­nif­i­cance Of Age-spe­cific Re­turn Sched­ules In Breed­ing Pur­ple Mar­tins. The Con­dor, 92: "1040-1050". Ac­cessed Sep­tem­ber 25, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​pss/​1368740.

Moss, W., J. Camin. 1970. Nest Par­a­sitism, Pro­duc­tiv­ity, and Clutch Size in Pur­ple Mar­tins. Sci­ence, 168/3934: "1000-1003".

Stutch­bury, B., J. Hill III, P. Krame, S. Rush, S. Tarof. 2009. Sex And Age-Spe­cific An­nual Sur­vival In A Neotrop­i­cal Mi­gra­tory Song­bird, The Pur­ple Mar­tin (Progne Subis). The Auk, 126/2: "278-287".

Turner, A. 1985. Swal­lows. Pp. "332-335" in C Per­rins, A Mid­dle­ton, eds. The En­cy­clo­pe­dia of Birds, Vol. I, I Edi­tion. New York: Facts on File.

Wade, J. 1966. What You Should Know About The Pur­ple Mar­tin. Grig­gsville, Illi­nois: J. L. Wade.

Wag­ner, R., M. Schug, E. Mor­ton. 1996. Con­di­tion-de­pen­dent con­trol of pa­ter­nity by fe­male pur­ple mar­tins: im­pli­ca­tions for colo­nial­ity. Be­hav­ioral Ecol­ogy and So­cio­bi­ol­ogy, 38/6: "379-389".

Williams, J. 1987. Field Me­tab­o­lism and Food Con­sump­tion of Sa­van­nah Spar­rows dur­ing the Breed­ing Sea­son. The Auk, 104/2: "277-289". Ac­cessed No­vem­ber 01, 2010 at http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​4087034.