Saguinus labiatusred-chested mustached tamarin

Ge­o­graphic Range

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins, Sagui­nus labi­a­tus, are found in South Amer­ica and are found in par­tic­u­larly high den­si­ties (up to 45 in­di­vid­u­als per sq km) in north­west­ern Bo­livia (Suarez 2007). Red-chested mus­tached tamarins also in­habit the mid­dle Ama­zon­ian re­gion of Brazil as well as south­east­ern Peru (Wolfheim 1983). (Suarez, 2007; Wolfheim, 1983)

Habi­tat

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins in­habit Ama­zon­ian rain­forests. They are ar­bo­real and spend most of their time in pri­mary and sec­ondary forests (Porter 2004, Mit­ter­meier and Wal­lace 2008). They are gen­er­ally found at el­e­va­tions be­tween 90 and 289 m. (Mit­ter­meier and Wal­lace, 2008; Porter, 2004; Suarez, 2007)

  • Range elevation
    90 to 289 m
    295.28 to 948.16 ft

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins are also known as red-bel­lied tamarins and white-lipped tamarins be­cause of their ap­pear­ance. They are mostly dark brown or black in color, and have dis­tin­guish­ing red mark­ings on their stom­achs and chests (Her­shkovitz 1977). They also have a patch of white fur sur­round­ing their nose and mouth, giv­ing the ap­pear­ance of a mus­tache (Her­shkovitz 1977). Red-chested mus­tached tamarins are rel­a­tively small, rang­ing from 23 to 29 cm in body length and 350 to 575 g in mass. Fe­males are gen­er­ally slightly larger than males (Suarez 2007). Mem­bers of this species have claw-like nails on all dig­its ex­cept the hal­lux, and their hind limbs are slightly longer than their fore­limbs (Gar­ber and Leigh 2001). The den­tal for­mula for this species is 2.​1.​3.​2 (Suarez 2007). (Gar­ber and Leigh, 2001; Her­skovitz, 1977; Suarez, 2007)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • sexes alike
  • female larger
  • Range mass
    450 to 575 g
    15.86 to 20.26 oz
  • Range length
    23 to 29 cm
    9.06 to 11.42 in

Re­pro­duc­tion

Be­cause breed­ing fe­males may mate with more than one male, red-chested mus­tached tamarins are gen­er­ally con­sid­ered polyan­drous. There is also ev­i­dence of male com­pe­ti­tion, as breed­ing males de­fend their mates by phys­i­cally in­sert­ing them­selves be­tween a breed­ing fe­male and non-breed­ing males within the group that ap­proach (Suarez 2007).

Re­cent stud­ies, how­ever, pro­pose a se­ri­ally monog­a­mous breed­ing sys­tem. Min­i­mal ag­gres­sion ob­served among males within a group has sup­ported this idea (Suarez 2007). Ad­di­tion­ally, ge­netic stud­ies of wild groups in­di­cate that one breed­ing male and one breed­ing fe­male are re­spon­si­ble for all in­fants and/or ju­ve­niles in a group (Suarez 2007). (Suarez, 2007)

Other adults in a group of red-chested mus­tached tamarins are con­sid­ered "helper" adults, and are typ­i­cally re­lated to one of the breed­ers (Suarez 2007). Dur­ing in­ter­group en­coun­ters, helper group mem­bers as­sess the breed­ing sit­u­a­tion of neigh­bor­ing groups. If a helper, or a pair of helpers such as a set of twins, senses in­sta­bil­ity in a neigh­bor­ing group, it may use an in­ter­group en­counter as an op­por­tu­nity to move to a dif­fer­ent group, in which they can at­tempt to be­come a breeder (Suarez 2007).

Breed­ing males also de­fend breed­ing fe­males dur­ing in­ter­group in­ter­ac­tions. Fe­males are gen­er­ally chased away by their mates from sites where in­ter­group in­ter­ac­tions occur. In fact, most ag­gres­sion of breed­ing male red-chested mus­tached tamarins is tar­geted at a breed­ing fe­male as he at­tempts to keep her away from these sites (Suarez 2007). (Suarez, 2007)

In the wild, red-chested mus­tached tamarins gen­er­ally mate in the spring (March to June), and breed­ing peeks in April. After a ges­ta­tion pe­riod of about 160 days, most in­fants are born in the fall (late Au­gust to early De­cem­ber) (Suarez 2007). In cap­tiv­ity, how­ever, mem­bers of this spe­ices can mate dur­ing any sea­son (Coates and Poole 1983). Red-chested mus­tached tamarins can breed as much as twice a year.

Each birth typ­i­cally pro­duces fra­ter­nal twins, each weigh­ing ap­prox­i­mately 40g, though oc­ca­sion­ally only a sin­gle in­fant is born (Nakamichi and Ya­mada 2009, Coates and Poole 1983). In­fants begin nurs­ing im­me­di­ately, and ob­tain all of their nu­tri­ents from their mother’s milk until they are 5 weeks old, when they may begin to eat solid food (Coates and Poole 1983). By 16 weeks of age, in­fants are usu­ally com­pletely weaned (Suarez 2007). Red-chested mus­tached tamarins be­come in­de­pen­dent around 1 to 2 years of age and sex­u­ally ma­ture around 2 to 4 years of age (Suarez 2007).

Breed­ing fe­males chem­i­cally in­hibit the first ovu­la­tion of their daugh­ters, as fe­males not re­lated to the pri­mary breed­ing fe­male are rarely tol­er­ated. These daugh­ters may reach sex­ual ma­tu­rity later than males of the same age (Smith and Gor­don 2002). (Coates and Poole, 1983; Nakamichi and Ya­mada, 2009; Smith and Gor­don, 2002; Suarez, 2007)

  • Breeding interval
    Breeding female red-chested mustached tamarins can breed up to twice yearly.
  • Breeding season
    In the wild, red-chested mustached tamarins tend to mate during the spring (March to June), particularly in April. In captivity, however, they mate during any season.
  • Range number of offspring
    1 to 2
  • Average number of offspring
    2
  • Range gestation period
    150 to 170 days
  • Average weaning age
    16 weeks
  • Range time to independence
    1 to 2 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    2 to 4 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    2 to 4 years

Be­cause red-chested mus­tached tamarins tend to live in groups, in­vest­ment in off­spring is shared. For 2 months after in­fants are born, a mother pro­vides nour­ish­ment to her young in the form of milk (Coates and Poole 1983). Other group mem­bers pro­tect in­fants by car­ry­ing them. This is gen­er­ally per­formed by the breed­ing male and fe­male as well as the helper male. Helper males are usu­ally re­lated to the de­gree of sib­ling or half-sib­ling to one of the breed­ing in­di­vid­u­als and thus gain in­clu­sive fit­ness by par­tic­i­pat­ing in in­fant care (Suarez 2007).

De­gree of in­vest­ment from each in­di­vid­ual varies from group to group. In some cases, the breed­ing male is re­spon­si­ble for most of the car­ry­ing (Pryce 1988), while in oth­ers, the breed­ing male and helper male equally con­tribute but carry much less often than the breed­ing fe­male (Suarez 2007). Other group mem­bers large enough to pro­vide in­fant care often do so, but not to the ex­tent of breed­ing adults or the most closely-re­lated helper male (Suarez 2007).

Once wean­ing oc­curs, in­fants begin to eat solid food. Be­cause in­fants are too small to for­age for their own food, they rely on older mem­bers of the group to share. In the wild, adult males ap­pear to be re­spon­si­ble for most food shar­ing, though the fa­ther does not nec­es­sar­ily share most (Suarez 2007). In­fants reach in­de­pen­dence around 1 to 2 years of age (Suarez 2007). (Coates and Poole, 1983; Pryce, 1988; Suarez, 2007)

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • male parental care
  • female parental care
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • male
    • protecting
      • male
      • female

Lifes­pan/Longevity

The lifes­pan of red-chested mus­tached tamarins is gen­er­ally un­known. Other mem­bers of the genus Sagui­nus live into their early teens (John­son 2008). In the wild, red-chested mus­tached tamarins es­ti­mated to be over the age of 8 based on tooth-wear are con­sid­ered “old” (Suarez 2007). (John­son, 2008; Suarez, 2007)

Be­hav­ior

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins are di­ur­nal, so­cial crea­tures that live in groups. Groups range in size from 2 to 13 in­di­vid­u­als, though groups typ­i­cally con­sist of 4 to 6 mem­bers (Suarez 2007, Buchanan-Smith 1991). Each group con­tains 1 breed­ing male and 1 breed­ing fe­male. The rest of the group is com­posed of off­spring and helper males, which are usu­ally re­lated to one of the breed­ing in­di­vid­u­als (Suarez 2007).

Often, groups of red-chested mus­tached tamarins as­so­ci­ate with groups of other cal­litrichines that share their home range. They have been ob­served as­so­ci­at­ing with groups of sad­dle-backed tamarins as well as groups of Goeldi's mar­mosets (Gar­ber and Leigh 2001). These as­so­ci­a­tions allow for in­creased vig­i­lance and home-range de­fense with­out in­creased com­pe­ti­tion for mates or food (Gar­ber and Leigh 2001; Hardie and Buchanan-Smith 1997). (Buchanan-Smith, 1991; Gar­ber and Leigh, 2001; Hardie and Buchanan-Smith, 1997; Suarez, 2007)

  • Range territory size
    0.15 to 0.23 km^2

Home Range

Ter­ri­to­ries of a group of red-chested mus­tached tamarins range from 0.15 to 0.23 sq km in size, but this may vary with sea­son and group size (Buchanan-Smith 1991; Suarez 2007). Red-chested mus­tached tamarins uti­lize 25 to 30% of their home range each day (Buchanan-Smith 1991). Some por­tions of the home range are vis­ited more fre­quently than oth­ers, par­tic­u­larly fa­vored feed­ing sites, sleep­ing sites, sites of in­ter­group en­coun­ters, and areas that pro­vide shel­ter from rain dur­ing the rainy sea­son. Red-chested mus­tached tamarins travel ap­prox­i­mately 1.3 to 1.7 km each day (Buchanan-Smith 1991). In densly pop­u­lated areas, home ranges of dif­fer­ent groups may over­lap by 20 to 30% (Suarez 2007). (Buchanan-Smith, 1991; Suarez, 2007)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins pri­mar­ily com­mu­ni­cate through scent mark­ing and vocal calls. They have scent glands in both the cir­cum­gen­i­tal and gu­larster­nal re­gions of their bod­ies (Suarez 2007). The size and use of these glands as well as the fre­quency of scent mark­ing vary with sex. Fe­male red-chested mus­tached tamarins tend to have larger an­gio­gen­i­tal and supra­pu­bic scent glands, and males scentv­mark using ster­nal glands more often than fe­males (Suarez 2007). Fe­males scent mark more fre­quently than males in al­most any cir­cum­stance (Smith and Gor­don 2002).

Fe­males use scent mark­ing more often when fer­tile. This scent mark­ing is thought to at­tract breed­ing males or com­mu­ni­cate re­cep­tive­ness to breed­ing (Suarez 2007), be­cause males, par­tic­u­larly breed­ing males, are much more in­ter­ested in check­ing fe­male scent mark­ings than fe­males are in check­ing male scent mark­ings (Gor­don and Smith 2002). Ad­di­tion­ally, breed­ing males are more likely to wit­ness (due to prox­im­ity), sniff, and over-mark a fe­male scent mark than other males (Suarez 2007).

Breed­ing males use scent mark­ing more often than other males in the group. Scent mark­ing by males oc­curs most often dur­ing in­ter­group en­coun­ters and thus may be a com­mu­ni­ca­tion of ter­ri­to­ri­al­ity (Suarez 2007). This rit­ual of male scent mark­ing re­quires two males (usu­ally the breed­ing male and the sec­ond-rank­ing male in the group), and be­gins with the males fac­ing one an­other. One male (usu­ally the breed­ing male) climbs over the other male and scent marks the bot­tom male’s head and back using an­gio­gen­i­tal scent glands. As this oc­curs, the bot­tom male scent marks the branch be­neath him with his ster­nal and then his supra­pu­bic scent glands (Suarez 2007). Once the top male has passed over the bot­tom male, he scent marks the branch be­hind the bot­tom male. Both con­clude the rit­ual with an an­gio­gen­i­tal scent mark (Suarez 2007). Be­cause this rit­ual gen­er­ally oc­curs dur­ing in­ter­group en­coun­ters, it may be a dis­play of male sol­i­dar­ity to ex­tra­group in­di­vid­u­als (Suarez 2007). (Smith and Gor­don, 2002; Suarez, 2007)

Be­cause they are small, red-chested mus­tached tamarins are con­stantly alert and per­form vi­sual scans of their en­vi­ron­ment quite often to en­sure they are safe from po­ten­tial preda­tors. Length of these vi­sual scans varies with group size (Hardie and Buchanan-Smith 2002). If a po­ten­tial preda­tor is seen, a tamarin is­sues a loud alarm call, which dif­fers based on the type of preda­tor (Suarez 2007).

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins have also been ob­served pro­duc­ing “long calls,” which seem to be calls to neigh­bor­ing groups of tamarins. These calls are often pro­duced when red-chested mus­tached tamarins emerge from their sleep­ing sites in the morn­ing and often re­sult in in­ter­group en­coun­ters (Suarez 2007). (Hardie and Buchanan-Smith, 1997; Suarez, 2007)

Food Habits

Al­though red-chested mus­tached tamarins are om­niv­o­rous, the ma­jor­ity of their diet (about 60%) con­sists of fruit (Porter 2001). They eat a va­ri­ety of fruits in­clud­ing Ce­cropia sci­ado­phylla, Pseudol­me­dia rigida, and ama­zon grapes, Pourouma ce­cropi­ae­fo­lia. How­ever, they pri­mar­ily eat fruits of the fam­ily Moraceae (Buchanan-Smith 1991).

Dur­ing the dry sea­son (June to Au­gust), when fruit is scarce, red-chested mus­tached tamarins often con­sume nec­tar, par­tic­u­larly from Sym­pho­nia glob­u­lif­erae and Ochroma pyr­mi­dale (Porter 2001). Mem­bers of this species also eat in­sects, pri­mar­ily from the fam­ily Or­thoptera (crick­ets, grasshop­pers), as well as plant ex­u­dates (Porter 2001). Red-chested mus­tached tamarins do not have large, procum­bent in­cisors, so their con­sump­tion of most ex­u­dates is likely op­por­tunis­tic. Ex­u­dates of Parkia pen­dula, how­ever, exude resin from a bean-shaped fruit, and preda­tors do not re­quire teeth that can dam­age the tough plant to ob­tain its nu­tri­ents (Buchanan-Smith 1991). (Buchanan-Smith, 1991; Porter, 2001)

  • Animal Foods
  • insects
  • Plant Foods
  • fruit
  • nectar
  • sap or other plant fluids

Pre­da­tion

Pre­da­tion of red-chested mus­tached tamarins is rarely ob­served in the wild. Crested ea­gles have been ob­served con­sum­ing in­fants of other spe­ices of tamarins (Vasquez and Hey­mann 2001). Po­ten­tial preda­tors are iden­ti­fied based on the alarm calls they elicit. An­i­mals known to elicit alarm calls from red-chested mus­tached tamarins in­clude: spec­ta­cled owls, great horned owls, tayras, ocelots, mar­gays, jaguarundi, jaguars, snakes of the fam­i­lies Boidae, Col­u­bri­dae, and Viperi­dae, as well as Ca­puchin mon­keys (Suarez 2007). (Suarez, 2007; Vasquez and Hey­mann, 2001)

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins de­crease their sus­cep­ti­bil­ity to pre­da­tion by form­ing groups, oc­ca­sion­ally with mem­bers of dif­fer­ent species. Multi-species groups usu­ally in­clude a group of an­other species of tamarin. Group for­ma­tion in­creases over­all vig­i­lance while de­creas­ing the amount of time each in­di­vid­ual must spend watch­ing for preda­tors (Hardie and Buchanan-Smith 1997). If a preda­tor is spot­ted, red-chested mus­tached tamarins pro­duce an alarm call to warn the oth­ers in their group (Suarez 2007).

The sleep­ing be­hav­ior of red-chested mus­tached tamarins also likely re­duces sus­cep­ti­bil­ity to pre­da­tion. Red-chested mus­tached tamarins choose sleep­ing sites high off the ground (12 to 20 m) where there are triple or quadru­ple forks in trees, aban­doned ter­mite mounds, or holes in trees so that they are not eas­ily vis­i­ble (Buchanan-Smith 1991). In ad­di­tion, these tamarins adopt a sleep­ing po­si­tion in which their head is tucked into their chest and their tail is wrapped around their body. This po­si­tion hides the white mark­ings on their face, mak­ing them more dif­fi­cult to spot (Buchanan-Smith 1991). (Buchanan-Smith, 1991; Hardie and Buchanan-Smith, 1997; Suarez, 2007)

Ecosys­tem Roles

As fru­gi­vores, red-chested mus­tached tamarins likely dis­perse seeds of fruit trees from which they eat. Seeds are swal­lowed (Fer­arri 1993) and may be ex­creted later to dis­perse seeds, as has been seen in other species of Sagui­nus (Oliviera and Fer­rari 2000). Red-chested mus­tached tamarins also act as prey for a va­ri­ety of Ama­zon­ian preda­tors.

In Peru, red-chested mus­tached tamarins act as hosts to the parasties Athesmia het­erolecithoides, Fi­laroides bar­retoi, Pri­ma­sub­u­lura jac­chi, and Pros­thenorchis el­e­gans (Michaud 2003). (Fer­rari, et al., 1993; Michaud, et al., 2003; Oliviera and Fer­rari, 2000)

  • Ecosystem Impact
  • disperses seeds
Com­men­sal/Par­a­sitic Species
  • Athesmia het­erolecithoides
  • Fi­laroides bar­retoi
  • Pri­ma­sub­u­lura jac­chi
  • Pros­thenorchis el­e­gans

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins do not often in­ter­act with hu­mans in the wild, though they are oc­ca­sion­ally hunted or taken as pets (Wolfheim 1983). In cap­tiv­ity, they have proven quite use­ful in sci­en­tific study. Mon­keys of the genus Sagui­nus, in­clud­ing red-chested mus­tached tamarins, are sus­cep­ti­ble to the strain of He­pati­tis A that af­fects hu­mans. A por­tion of what we have learned re­gard­ing the pathol­ogy of He­pati­tis A has stemmed from the study of the dis­ease in these an­i­mals (Karayian­nis 1986). (Karayian­nis, et al., 1986; Wolfheim, 1983)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

Red-chested mus­tached tamarins do not often in­ter­act with hu­mans, though it is pos­si­ble that, when in cap­tiv­ity, these an­i­mals could trans­mit par­a­sites to hu­mans (Michaud 2003). (Michaud, et al., 2003)

  • Negative Impacts
  • injures humans
    • carries human disease

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

Pop­u­la­tions of red-chested mus­tached tamarins are sta­ble, and they are not con­sid­ered threat­ened. How­ever, it is pos­si­ble that de­for­esta­tion in Bo­livia could re­duce habi­tat (Mit­ter­meier and Wal­lace 2008). This species is not often hunted, though red-chested mus­tached tamarins are oc­ca­sion­ally be taken as pets (Mit­ter­meier and Wal­lace 2008). (Mit­ter­meier and Wal­lace, 2008)

Other Com­ments

A very re­cent study based on mi­to­chon­dr­ial ri­bo­so­mal RNA has re­vealed that red-chested mus­tached tamarins, Sagui­nus labi­a­tus, and mus­tached tamarins, S. mys­tax, are sis­ter taxa and only di­verged from one an­other ap­prox­i­mately 1.15 mil­lion years ago (Matauschek, Roos, and Hey­mann 2011). (Matauschek, et al., 2011)

Con­trib­u­tors

Nina Kristofik (au­thor), Yale Uni­ver­sity, Eric Sar­gis (ed­i­tor), Yale Uni­ver­sity, Gail Mc­Cormick (ed­i­tor), An­i­mal Di­ver­sity Web Staff.

Glossary

Neotropical

living in the southern part of the New World. In other words, Central and South America.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

arboreal

Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

female parental care

parental care is carried out by females

fertilization

union of egg and spermatozoan

food

A substance that provides both nutrients and energy to a living thing.

frugivore

an animal that mainly eats fruit

herbivore

An animal that eats mainly plants or parts of plants.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

male parental care

parental care is carried out by males

monogamous

Having one mate at a time.

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

pet trade

the business of buying and selling animals for people to keep in their homes as pets.

polyandrous

Referring to a mating system in which a female mates with several males during one breeding season (compare polygynous).

rainforest

rainforests, both temperate and tropical, are dominated by trees often forming a closed canopy with little light reaching the ground. Epiphytes and climbing plants are also abundant. Precipitation is typically not limiting, but may be somewhat seasonal.

scent marks

communicates by producing scents from special gland(s) and placing them on a surface whether others can smell or taste them

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

territorial

defends an area within the home range, occupied by a single animals or group of animals of the same species and held through overt defense, display, or advertisement

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

Ref­er­ences

Buchanan-Smith, H. 1991. A Field Study on the Red-Bel­lied Tamarin, Sagui­nus labi­a­tus labi­a­tus, in Bo­livia. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 12/3: 259-276.

Coates, A., T. Poole. 1983. The Be­hav­ior of the Cal­litrichid Mon­key, Sagui­nus labi­a­tus labi­a­tus, in the Lab­o­ra­tory. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 4/4: 339-371.

Fer­rari, S., M. Lopes, E. Krause. 1993. Gut mor­phol­ogy of Cal­lithrix ni­gri­ceps and Sagui­nus labi­a­tus from west­ern Brazil­ian Ama­zo­nia. Jour­nal of Amer­i­can Phys­i­cal An­thro­pol­ogy, 90/4: 487-493.

Gar­ber, P., S. Leigh. 2001. Pat­terns of Po­si­tional Be­hav­ior in Mixed-Species Troops of Cal­lim­ico goeldii, Sagui­nus labi­a­tus, and Sagui­nus fus­ci­col­lis in North­west­ern Brazil. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 54: 17-31.

Hardie, S., H. Buchanan-Smith. 1997. Vig­i­lance in Sin­gle- and Mixed-Species Groups of Tamarins (Sagui­nus labi­a­tus and Sagui­nus fus­ci­col­lis). In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 18/2: 217-234.

Her­skovitz, P. 1977. Liv­ing New World mon­keys (Platyrrhini): with an In­tro­duc­tion to Pri­mates. Chicago, Il: Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

John­son, D. 2008. "The Life Spans of Non­hu­man Pri­mates" (On-line). Pri­mate Info Net. Ac­cessed March 16, 2011 at http://​pin.​primate.​wisc.​edu/​aboutp/​phys/​lifespan.​html.

Karayian­nis, P., T. Jowett, M. En­ti­cott, D. Moore, M. Pig­natelli, F. Brenes, P. Scheuer, H. Thomas. 1986. He­pati­tis A Virus Repli­ca­tion in Tamarins and Host Im­mune Re­sponse in Re­la­tion to Patho­gen­e­sis of Liver Cell Dam­age. Jour­nal of Med­ical Vi­rol­ogy, 18/3: 261-276.

Matauschek, C., C. Roos, E. Hey­mann. 2011. Mi­to­chon­dr­ial Phy­logeny of Tamarins (Sagui­nus, Hoff­mannsegg 1807) with Tax­o­nomic and Bio­geo­graphic Im­pli­ca­tions for the S. ni­gri­col­lis Species Group. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Phys­i­cal An­thro­pol­ogy, 144: 564–574.

Michaud, C., M. Tan­ta­lean, C. Ique, E. Mon­toya, A. Gon­zalo. 2003. A sur­vey for helminth par­a­sites in feral New World non-hu­man pri­mate pop­u­la­tions and its com­par­i­son with par­a­sito­log­i­cal data from man in the re­gion. Jour­nal of Med­ical Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 32: 341-345.

Mit­ter­meier, R., R. Wal­lace. 2008. "Sagui­nus labi­a­tus" (On-line). IUCN Red List of Threat­ened Species. Ver­sion 2010.4. Ac­cessed April 17, 2011 at http://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​apps/​redlist/​details/​41524/​0.

Nakamichi, M., K. Ya­mada. 2009. Dis­tri­b­u­tion of dor­sal car­riage among simi­ans. Pri­mates, 50: 153-168.

Oliviera, A., S. Fer­rari. 2000. Seed dis­per­sal by black-handed tamarins, Sagui­nus midas niger (Cal­litrichi­nae, Pri­mates): im­pli­ca­tions for the re­gen­er­a­tion of de­graded for­est habi­tats in east­ern Ama­zo­nia. Jour­nal of Trop­i­cal Ecol­ogy, 16: 709-716.

Porter, L. 2001. Di­etary Dif­fer­ences Among Sym­patric Cal­litrichi­nae in North­ern Bo­livia: Cal­lim­ico goeldii, Sagui­nus fus­ci­col­lis and S. labi­a­tus. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 22/6: 961-992.

Porter, L. 2004. For­est Use and Ac­tiv­ity Pat­terns of Cal­lim­ico goeldii in Com­par­i­son to Two Sym­patric Tamarins, Sagui­nus fus­ci­col­lis and Sagui­nus labi­a­tus. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Phys­i­cal An­thro­pol­ogy, 124: 139-153.

Pryce, C. 1988. In­di­vid­ual and group ef­fects on early care­giver-in­fant re­la­tion­ships in red-bel­lied tamarin mon­keys. An­i­mal Be­hav­ior, 36: 1455-1464.

Smith, T., S. Gor­don. 2002. Sex Dif­fer­ences in Ol­fac­tory Com­mu­ni­ca­tion in Sagui­nus labi­a­tus. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 23/2: 429-441.

Suarez, S. 2007. Pa­ter­nity, Re­lat­ed­ness, and So­cio-Re­pro­duc­tive Be­hav­ior in a Pop­u­la­tion of Wild Red-Bel­lied Tamarins (Sagui­nus labi­a­tus). Ann Arbor, Michi­gan: Pro­Quest In­for­ma­tion and Learn­ing Com­pany.

Vasquez, M., E. Hey­mann. 2001. Crested Eagle (Mor­phnus guia­nen­sis) Pre­da­tion on In­fant Tamarins (Sagui­nus mys­tax and Sagui­nus fus­ci­col­lis, Cal­litrichi­nae). Folia Pri­ma­to­log­ica, 72: 301-303.

Wolfheim, J. 1983. Pri­mates of the World: Dis­tri­b­u­tion, Abun­dance and Con­ser­va­tion. Seat­tle: Uni­ver­sity of Wash­ing­ton Press.