Tursiops aduncusIndo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin

Ge­o­graphic Range

Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins tend to live in shal­low water near the shore at depths of less than 300 m. The habi­tat of some Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins is es­tu­ar­ine but spa­tial dis­tri­b­u­tion of dol­phins varies de­pend­ing on sea­son and tidal state. These vari­ables in­flu­ence water tem­per­a­ture and prey dis­tri­b­u­tion. (Fury and Har­ri­son, 2008; Fury and Har­ri­son, 2011; Kogi, et al., 2004; Kuri­hara and Oda, 2007; Nowak, 2003; Saay­man, et al., 1973; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006)

Habi­tat

Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins tend to live in shal­low water near the shore at depths of less than 300 m. With rapid gas ex­change at the cap­il­lar­ies, dou­ble the amount of ery­thro­cytes, and about 2 to 9 times the amount of myo­glo­bin of land an­i­mals, dol­phins are able to al­ter­nate be­tween no breath­ing while deep div­ing and nor­mal breath­ing while swim­ming along the sur­face. (Fury and Har­ri­son, 2011; Kogi, et al., 2004; Mar­tin, et al., 2001; Nowak, 2003; Vaughan, et al., 2011)

  • Range depth
    2 to 300 m
    6.56 to 984.25 ft

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Tur­siops adun­cus are sim­i­lar to Tur­siops trun­ca­tus in form with a fusiform body, dor­sal fin, and beak. The dor­sal sur­face is slate blue or dark gray, with darker flip­pers and ex­trem­i­ties and lighter, often pink-tinted, un­der­sides. Pat­tern­ing and ven­tral spot­ting vary by age and ge­o­graphic lo­ca­tion. A re­cent study has claimed that ven­tral spot­ting may be a sign of re­pro­duc­tive ma­tu­rity, es­pe­cially in fe­males. (Krzyszczyk and Mann, 2012; Nowak, 2003; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006; Vaughan, et al., 2011)

Adult Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins have a head and body length be­tween 175 and 400 cm, a pec­toral fin length of about 23 cm, and a tail fluke ex­panse of 60 cm. Their weight is about 230 kg. Adult fe­male Tur­siops adun­cus have been mea­sured to be 200 cm in length in cer­tain re­gions, while adult males are usu­ally longer and heav­ier than fe­males. Fe­males have a sin­gle ex­ter­nal open­ing and males have dis­tinct anal and gen­i­tal open­ings. (Kogi, et al., 2004; Mann, et al., 2000; Nowak, 2003; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006; Vaughan, et al., 2011)

Like most bot­tlenosed dol­phins, Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenose dol­phins have con­i­cal, sin­gle-rooted, uni­cus­pid, ho­mod­ont teeth that are about 1 cm in di­am­e­ter. How­ever, tooth counts alone usu­ally can­not be used to dif­fer­en­ti­ate T. adun­cus and T. trun­ca­tus. In gen­eral, bot­tlenosed dol­phins have be­tween 20 and 28 teeth on each side of the jaw. An­other cra­nial fea­ture is the con­cav­ity of the top of the ros­trum be­tween the an­te­rior edge of the nares and the dis­tal tip of the pre­max­il­lae. (Kuri­hara and Oda, 2007; Mar­tin, et al., 2001; Nowak, 2003)

There are a num­ber of phys­i­cal dif­fer­ences be­tween T. trun­ca­tus and T. adun­cus. Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins can be dis­tin­guished from T. trun­ca­tus by ven­tral spot­ting in adult spec­i­mens which in­creases with age, a longer, bet­ter-de­fined ros­trum, a smaller melon, and, in some cases, more teeth. Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins also have a smaller body, a smaller head, and larger flip­pers than T. trun­ca­tus. They have a more slen­der ros­trum that is ta­pered more abruptly near the base and taller and broader-based dor­sal fins rel­a­tive to other bot­tlenosed dol­phins. How­ever, these dif­fer­ences are vari­able, which can make it dif­fi­cult to dis­tin­guish them in areas of over­lap. (Kogi, et al., 2004; Kuri­hara and Oda, 2007; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006)

With rapid gas ex­change at the cap­il­lar­ies, dou­ble the amount of ery­thro­cytes, and about 2 to 9 times the amount of myo­glo­bin of land an­i­mals, dol­phins are able to al­ter­nate be­tween no breath­ing while deep div­ing and nor­mal breath­ing while swim­ming along the sur­face. (Mar­tin, et al., 2001)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • male larger
  • Average mass
    230 kg
    506.61 lb
  • Range length
    175 to 400 cm
    68.90 to 157.48 in

Re­pro­duc­tion

Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are one of the few mam­mal species in which males co­op­er­ate with other males to allow for eas­ier mat­ing with fe­males. Males form al­liances with one to three other , po­ten­tially un­re­lated, males. These male groups herd fe­males for mat­ing, some­times called “mate guard­ing.” Sin­gle males may also at­tempt to guard fe­males for mat­ing. Breed­ing fe­males also form groups. Smaller fe­male groups are eas­ier to de­fend, whereas larger groups of fe­males are dif­fi­cult to de­fend. Male and fe­male dol­phins tend to mate with more than one part­ner. Cop­u­la­tion usu­ally oc­curs when the dol­phins are po­si­tioned belly to belly in the same di­rec­tion. (Möller, et al., 2001; Reynolds, et al., 2000)

Fe­male re­pro­duc­tive suc­cess de­pends on the depth of the water; shal­low water al­lows for eas­ier de­tec­tion of preda­tors and re­duced pre­da­tion over­all by sharks. Fe­males reach re­pro­duc­tive ma­tu­rity be­tween 7 and 12 years of age, this mat­u­ra­tion is com­mu­ni­cated to males by freck­ling on the ven­tral re­gion. Males reach re­pro­duc­tive ma­tu­rity be­tween 9 and 13 years. The ges­ta­tion pe­riod is about 12 months. (Krzyszczyk and Mann, 2012; Mann, et al., 2000; Nowak, 2003; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006)

At birth, Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are be­tween 0.8 and 1.1 m in length and be­tween 9 and 21 kg. Young are born tail first and are able to swim im­me­di­ately. The high­est rates of births are from Oc­to­ber to De­cem­ber. The lac­ta­tion pe­riod lasts for about 18 months in cap­tiv­ity and about 32 months in the wild. The teats are en­closed in slits along the uro­gen­i­tal open­ing. The mean wean­ing age is 3.5 years. How­ever, a study in Aus­tralia found a wean­ing age rang­ing from 2.7 to 8 years. Adult fe­males, in a sam­ple pop­u­la­tion off the coast of Mikura Is­land, Japan, give birth once every 3 to 4 years. (Kogi, et al., 2004; Mann, et al., 2000; Nowak, 2003; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006; Vaughan, et al., 2011)

  • Breeding interval
    Female Indo-Pacific bottlenosed dolphins breed every 4 to 6 years.
  • Breeding season
    The breeding season typically ranges from September to January. However, breeding may occur throughout the year.
  • Average number of offspring
    1
  • Average gestation period
    12 months
  • Average weaning age
    42 months
  • Average time to independence
    4 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    7 to 12 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    9 to 13 years

Adult fe­male Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins in­vest a great deal of time and at­ten­tion in their calves. Even after wean­ing, calves stay with their moth­ers for an­other one to three years. (Mann, et al., 2000; Reynolds, et al., 2000; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006)

Adult male Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins pro­vide lit­tle or no parental care, in­stead max­i­miz­ing their mat­ing op­por­tu­ni­ties. Males some­times co­op­er­ate to de­fend groups of fe­males. (Möller, et al., 2001)

  • Parental Investment
  • precocial
  • pre-fertilization
    • provisioning
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • post-independence association with parents

Lifes­pan/Longevity

The av­er­age lifes­pan of the Indo-Pa­cific Bot­tlenose dol­phin in the wild is more than 40 years, while the av­er­age age of adult males and fe­males in the wild is 19 years and 26 years, re­spec­tively. The old­est known dol­phins in the wild are a 39 year old male and a 49 year old fe­male. (Kli­nowska and Cook, 1991; Nowak, 2003; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006)

  • Range lifespan
    Status: wild
    39 for male, 49 for female (high) years
  • Average lifespan
    Status: wild
    40+ years

Be­hav­ior

Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are so­cially and be­hav­iorally sim­i­lar to com­mon bot­tlenosed dol­phins and oc­ca­sion­ally form pods with com­mon bot­tlenosed dol­phins and less often with Indo-Pa­cific hump­back dol­phins. Most Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenose dol­phins spend their lives in schools or pods of be­tween 5 and 15 dol­phins to help with hunt­ing, preda­tor avoid­ance, and re­pro­duc­tion. They have been ob­served, in the wild and in cap­tiv­ity, play­ing by chas­ing and splash­ing each other and some have linked this be­hav­ior with learn­ing. In these pods, there is a so­cial hi­er­ar­chy where the largest dol­phin is usu­ally the most dom­i­nant. In the wild there are also same sex groups, es­pe­cially in subadult groups, that later join larger pods. They are usu­ally less so­cial and more shy than com­mon bot­tlenosed dol­phins. They are less fre­quently ob­served at the sur­face and make less fre­quent leaps from the water. (Nowak, 2003; Reynolds, et al., 2000; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006; Vaughan, et al., 2011)

  • Range territory size
    15 to 85 km^2

Home Range

Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins ex­hibit site fi­delity and have well-es­tab­lished home ranges. Most males and fe­males re­main in their natal home range until ma­tu­rity. In the Pa­cific, home ranges tend to be found around an is­land. Home ranges can be up to 85 square km, how­ever the range de­pends on gen­der and may change sea­son­ally. Den­sity es­ti­mates are re­ported to range from 0.06 to 4.80 per sq km. (Möller, et al., 2001; Nowak, 2003)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

It has been ar­gued that the large and con­vo­luted brain of Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins al­lows for greater in­tel­li­gence and higher-or­der learn­ing. They com­mu­ni­cate via au­di­tory per­cep­tion. While their sense of sight is not well-de­vel­oped, their sense of hear­ing is keen and im­por­tant in echolo­ca­tion. Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins gen­er­ate ul­tra­sonic clicks and in­ter­pret the re­turn­ing sig­nals to per­ceive ob­jects in their en­vi­ron­ment. Each dol­phin also uses a char­ac­ter­is­tic whis­tle that helps other iden­tify them in­di­vid­u­ally. Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins also com­mu­ni­cate via tac­tile sig­nals. Most often, flip­pers are rubbed over the flip­pers or bod­ies of other dol­phins of the same sex and age. Flip­per rub­bing is ob­served be­tween op­po­site sexes around mat­ing and be­tween moth­ers and calves. (Mar­tin, et al., 2001; Nowak, 2003; Sakai, et al., 2006; Vaughan, et al., 2011)

Food Habits

Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins have a diet con­sist­ing of mainly bony fish and, to a lesser de­gree, cephalopods. While they eat a wide va­ri­ety of fish species, the ma­jor­ity of their diet is com­posed of just a few species, which varies re­gion­ally. In cap­tiv­ity these dol­phins eat 6 to 7 kg of food per day. They hunt co­op­er­a­tively and for­ag­ing be­hav­ior is char­ac­ter­ized by shal­low dives mul­ti­ple times per minute. In shal­low water they hunt by using sev­eral meth­ods, in­clud­ing “kick­ing” fish into the sand with their tails and chas­ing small fish up on to the shore. While feed­ing and trav­el­ing they leap from the water reg­u­larly. Hunt­ing and feed­ing occur most fre­quently in the morn­ing and af­ter­noon. (Amir, et al., 2005; Nowak, 2003; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006; Vaughan, et al., 2011)

  • Animal Foods
  • fish
  • mollusks

Pre­da­tion

Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are hunted by at least 10 species of sharks. These preda­tors may have played an im­por­tant role in the evo­lu­tion of so­cial be­hav­ior in bot­tlenosed dol­phins. By trav­el­ing in groups, they are less vul­ner­a­ble to preda­tors. Hu­mans are also preda­tors be­cause bot­tlenosed dol­phins are hunted and cap­tured for food and en­ter­tain­ment. Their coun­ter­shaded color pat­tern also helps to make them harder to see in ma­rine en­vi­ron­ments. (Hei­thaus, 2001; Mann, et al., 2000; Reynolds, et al., 2000)

  • Anti-predator Adaptations
  • cryptic

Ecosys­tem Roles

Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are preda­tors of many species of bony fish and squid in coastal trop­i­cal wa­ters. Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are prey of many species of sharks. They often act as hosts for par­a­sites and bar­na­cles. Semi-par­a­sitic bar­na­cles can at­tach to their skin, caus­ing ir­ri­ta­tions and drag as they swim. (Amir, et al., 2005; Hei­thaus, 2001; Per­rin, et al., 2008)

Com­men­sal/Par­a­sitic Species

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

At some sites, Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins have been ha­bit­u­ated to the pres­ence of human swim­mers. At Mikura Is­land, Japan, there is a dol­phin swim pro­gram in the sum­mer that began in the 1990’s. Bot­tlenosed dol­phins are com­mon in ma­rine ex­hibits and zoos. They can be eas­ily trained to per­form agile dis­plays and to play with and lo­cate ob­jects. How­ever, Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are gen­er­ally more shy and less in­quis­i­tive than other bot­tlenosed dol­phins. Like other ma­rine mam­mals, the lifes­pan of these dol­phins in cap­tiv­ity is sig­nif­i­cantly shorter than in the wild and there are sig­nif­i­cant eth­i­cal con­cerns with keep­ing such large, in­tel­li­gent mam­mals in cap­tive con­di­tions. (Kogi, et al., 2004; Mar­tin, et al., 2001; Nowak, 2003; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

There are no known ad­verse ef­fects of Tur­siops adun­cus on hu­mans.

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

Hunt­ing and shark nets have his­tor­i­cally threat­ened pop­u­la­tions of bot­tlenosed dol­phins be­cause these dol­phins, es­pe­cially Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins, pre­fer shal­low coastal wa­ters. These dol­phins are killed for meat, fer­til­izer, cook­ing oil, and ma­chine lu­bri­ca­tion. (Ham­mond, et al., 2008; Lip­scomb, et al., 1994; Nowak, 2003; Reynolds, et al., 2000; Van Bressem, et al., 2001)

Other Com­ments

All Tur­siops species were ini­tially in­cluded in the species Tur­siops trun­ca­tus. It is now rec­og­nized that there are at least three species, in­clud­ing Tur­siops trun­ca­tus (found in all oceans ex­cept polar wa­ters), Tur­siops gillii (found in the east­ern North Pa­cific), and Tur­siops adun­cus (found in the In­dian, South Pa­cific, and west­ern and south­ern North Pa­cific oceans). Tur­siops adun­cus was iden­ti­fied in 1883 by Chris­t­ian Ehren­berg. Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are dif­fer­en­ti­ated from com­mon bot­tlenosed dol­phins by ge­netic and mor­pho­log­i­cal dif­fer­ences. Some have sug­gested that Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenosed dol­phins are more closely re­lated to Stenella and Del­phi­nus species than to Tur­siops trun­ca­tus. This is an area of ac­tive re­search. (Nowak, 2003; Reynolds, et al., 2000; Rice, 1998; Shir­i­hai and Jar­rett, 2006)

Con­trib­u­tors

Kelly Diaz (au­thor), Yale Uni­ver­sity, Rachel Raci­cot (ed­i­tor), Yale Uni­ver­sity, Eric Sar­gis (ed­i­tor), Yale Uni­ver­sity, Tanya Dewey (ed­i­tor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor.

Glossary

Pacific Ocean

body of water between the southern ocean (above 60 degrees south latitude), Australia, Asia, and the western hemisphere. This is the world's largest ocean, covering about 28% of the world's surface.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

brackish water

areas with salty water, usually in coastal marshes and estuaries.

carnivore

an animal that mainly eats meat

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

coastal

the nearshore aquatic habitats near a coast, or shoreline.

cooperative breeder

helpers provide assistance in raising young that are not their own

cryptic

having markings, coloration, shapes, or other features that cause an animal to be camouflaged in its natural environment; being difficult to see or otherwise detect.

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
dominance hierarchies

ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates

echolocation

The process by which an animal locates itself with respect to other animals and objects by emitting sound waves and sensing the pattern of the reflected sound waves.

ecotourism

humans benefit economically by promoting tourism that focuses on the appreciation of natural areas or animals. Ecotourism implies that there are existing programs that profit from the appreciation of natural areas or animals.

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

estuarine

an area where a freshwater river meets the ocean and tidal influences result in fluctuations in salinity.

food

A substance that provides both nutrients and energy to a living thing.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

keystone species

a species whose presence or absence strongly affects populations of other species in that area such that the extirpation of the keystone species in an area will result in the ultimate extirpation of many more species in that area (Example: sea otter).

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

natatorial

specialized for swimming

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

piscivore

an animal that mainly eats fish

polygynandrous

the kind of polygamy in which a female pairs with several males, each of which also pairs with several different females.

reef

structure produced by the calcium carbonate skeletons of coral polyps (Class Anthozoa). Coral reefs are found in warm, shallow oceans with low nutrient availability. They form the basis for rich communities of other invertebrates, plants, fish, and protists. The polyps live only on the reef surface. Because they depend on symbiotic photosynthetic algae, zooxanthellae, they cannot live where light does not penetrate.

saltwater or marine

mainly lives in oceans, seas, or other bodies of salt water.

sedentary

remains in the same area

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

ultrasound

uses sound above the range of human hearing for either navigation or communication or both

vibrations

movements of a hard surface that are produced by animals as signals to others

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

year-round breeding

breeding takes place throughout the year

young precocial

young are relatively well-developed when born

Ref­er­ences

Amir, O., P. Berggren, S. Ndaro, N. Jid­dawi. 2005. Feed­ing ecol­ogy of the Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenose dol­phin (Tur­siops adun­cus) in­ci­den­tally caught in the gill­net fish­eries off Zanz­ibar, Tan­za­nia. Es­tu­ar­ine, Coastal and Shelf Sci­ence, 63/3: 429-437.

Fury, C., P. Har­ri­son. 2008. Abun­dance, site fi­delity, and range pat­terns of Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus) in two Aus­tralian sub­trop­i­cal es­tu­ar­ies. Ma­rine and Fresh­wa­ter Re­search, 59/11: 1015-1027.

Fury, C., P. Har­ri­son. 2011. Sea­sonal vari­a­tion and tidal in­flu­ences on es­tu­ar­ine use by bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus). Es­tu­ar­ine, Coastal and Shelf Sci­ence, 93/4: 389-395.

Ham­mond, P., G. Bearzi, A. Bjørge, K. For­ney, L. Kar­cz­marski, T. Ka­suya, W. Per­rin, M. Scott, J. Wang, R. Wells, B. Wil­son. 2008. "Tur­siops adun­cus" (On-line). IUCN Red List of Threat­ened Species. Ac­cessed Feb­ru­ary 10, 2012 at www.​iucnredlist.​org.

Hei­thaus, M. 2001. Shark At­tacks on Bot­tlenose Dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus) in Shark Bay, West­ern Aus­tralia: At­tack Rate, Bite Scar Fre­quen­cies, and At­tack Sea­son­al­ity. Ma­rine Mam­mal Sci­ence, 17/3: 526-539.

Kli­nowska, M., J. Cook. 1991. Dol­phins, Por­poises and Whales of the World: The IUCN Red Data Book. Cam­bridge, Eng­land: IUCN.

Kogi, K., T. Hishii, A. Ima­mura, T. Iwatani, K. Dudzin­ski. 2004. De­mo­graphic pa­ra­me­ters of indo-pa­cific bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus) around Mikura Is­land, Japan. Ma­rine Mam­mal Sci­ence, 20/3: 510-526.

Krzyszczyk, E., J. Mann. 2012. Why be­come speck­led? On­togeny and func­tion of speck­ling in Shark Bay bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops spp.). Ma­rine Mam­mal Sci­ence, 28/2: 295-307.

Kuri­hara, N., S. Oda. 2007. Cra­nial vari­a­tion in bot­tlenose dol­phins Tur­siops spp. from the In­dian and west­ern Pa­cific Oceans: ad­di­tional ev­i­dence for two Species. Acta The­ri­o­log­ica, 52/4: 403-418.

Lal Mohan, R. 1982. A Note on the Fe­tuses of the Dol­phins Tur­siops adun­cus and Sousa plumbea from the South­west Coast of India. In­dian Jour­nal of Fish­eries, 29/1-2: 249-252.

Lal Mohan, R. 1985. Os­te­ol­ogy of dol­phins Del­phi­nus del­phis trop­i­calis, Stenella lon­girostris, Tur­siops adun­cus and Sousa chi­nen­sis from south­west coast of India. Sym­po­sium on En­dan­gered Ma­rine An­i­mals and Ma­rine Parks, 1: 1-10.

Lip­scomb, T., F. Schul­man, D. Mof­fett, S. Kennedy. 1994. Mor­bil­livi­ral Dis­ease in At­lantic Bot­tlenose Dol­phins (Tur­siops trun­ca­tus) from the 1987-1988 Epi­zootic. Jour­nal of Wildlife Dis­eases., 30/4: 567-571.

Mann, J., R. Con­nor, L. Barre, M. Hei­thaus. 2000. Fe­male re­pro­duc­tive suc­cess in bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops sp.): life his­tory, habi­tat, pro­vi­sion­ing, and group-size ef­fects. Be­hav­ioral Ecol­ogy, 11/2: 210-219.

Mar­tin, R., R. Pine, A. De­Blase. 2001. A Man­ual of Mam­mal­ogy with Keys to Fam­i­lies of the World. Long Grove, IL: Wave­land Press, Inc.

Möller, L., L. Be­here­garay, R. Har­court, M. Krützen. 2001. Al­liance mem­ber­ship and kin­ship in wild male bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus) of south­east­ern Aus­tralia. Pro­ceed­ings of the Royal So­ci­ety of Bi­o­log­i­cal Sci­ences, 268: 1941-1947.

Na­toli, A., V. Ped­de­mors, A. Hoelzel. 2008. Pop­u­la­tion struc­ture of bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus) im­pacted by by­catch along the east coast of South Africa. Con­ser­va­tion Ge­net­ics, 9/3: 627-636.

Nowak, R. 2003. Walker's Ma­rine Mam­mals of the World. Bal­ti­more, MD: John Hop­kins Uni­ver­sity Press.

Per­rin, W., B. Würsig, J. Thewis­sen. 2008. En­cy­clo­pe­dia of Ma­rine Mam­mals. San Diego, CA: Aca­d­e­mic Press.

Reynolds, J., R. Wells, S. Eide. 2000. The Bot­tlenose Dol­phin: Bi­ol­ogy and Con­ser­va­tion. Florida: Uni­ver­sity Press of Florida.

Rice, D. 1998. Ma­rine Mam­mals of the World: Sys­tem­at­ics and Dis­tri­b­u­tion. San Fran­cisco, CA: So­ci­ety for Ma­rine Mam­mal­ogy.

Robaae, K. 1974. Tur­siops adun­cus bot­tle nosed dol­phin: a new record for Arab Gulf; with notes on Cetacea of the re­gion. Bul­letin of the Bas­rah Nat­ural His­tory Mu­seum, 1/1: 7-16.

Saay­man, G., C. Tayler. 1973. So­cial Or­ga­ni­za­tion of In­shore Dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus and g. Sousa) in the In­dian Ocean. Amer­i­can So­ci­ety of Mam­mal­o­gists, 54/4: 993-996.

Saay­man, G., C. Tayler, D. Bower. 1973. Di­ur­nal Ac­tiv­ity Cy­cles in Cap­tive and Free-Rang­ing In­dian Ocean Bot­tlenose Dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus Ehren­burg). Be­hav­ior, 44/3-4: 212-232.

Sakai, M., T. Hishii, S. Takeda, S. Kohshima. 2006. Flip­per rub­bing be­hav­iors in wild bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus). Ma­rine Mam­mal Sci­ence, 22/4: 966-978.

Shir­i­hai, H., B. Jar­rett. 2006. Whales, Dol­phins, and Other Ma­rine Mam­mals of the World. Prince­ton, NJ: Prince­ton Uni­ver­sity Press.

Stens­land, E., P. Berggren. 2007. Be­hav­ioural changes in fe­male Indo-Pa­cific bot­tlenose dol­phins in re­sponse to boat-based tourism. Ma­rine Ecol­ogy-Progress Se­ries, 332: 225-234.

Tayler, C., G. Saay­man. 1973. Im­i­ta­tive Be­hav­ior by In­dian Ocean Bot­tlenose Dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus) in Cap­tiv­ity. Be­hav­ior, 44/3-4: 286-298.

Van Bressem, M., K. Waere­beek, P. Jep­son, J. Raga, P. Duig­nan, O. Nielsen, A. DiBeneditto, S. Si­cil­iano, R. Ramos, W. Kant, V. Ped­de­mors, R. Ki­noshita, P. Ross, A. López-Fer­nan­dez, K. Evans, E. Cre­spo, T. Bar­rett. 2001. An in­sight into the epi­demi­ol­ogy of dol­phin mor­bil­livirus world­wide. Vet­eri­nary Mi­cro­bi­ol­ogy, 81/4: 287-304.

Vaughan, T., J. Ryan, N. Czaplewski. 2011. Mam­mal­ogy. Boston, MA: Jones & Bartlett Learn­ing.

Wahlberg, M., F. Jensen, N. Soto, K. Beed­holm, L. Be­jder, C. Oliveira, M. Ras­mussen, M. Simon, A. Vil­lads­gaard, P. Mad­sen. 2011. Source Pa­ra­me­ters of echolo­ca­tion clicks from wild bot­tlenose dol­phins (Tur­siops adun­cus and Tur­siops trun­ca­tus). Jour­nal of the Acousti­cal So­ci­ety of Amer­ica, 130/4: 2263-2274.

Whit­low, W., R. Floyd, R. Pen­ner, A. Murchi­son. 1974. Mea­sure­ment of echolo­ca­tion sig­nals of the At­lantic bot­tlenose dol­phin, Tur­siops trun­ca­tus Mon­tagu, in open wa­ters. Jour­nal of the Acousti­cal So­ci­ety of Amer­ica, 56/4: 1280-1290.